Contact call

Contact calls are seemingly haphazard sounds made by many social animals (such as a chicken's cluck).[1] Contact calls are unlike other calls (such as alarm calls) in that they are not usually widely used, conspicuous calls, but rather short exclamations that differ between individuals. Often, the message that the call is meant to convey is specific to the individual or group's activity, such as informing other members of the group about one's location while foraging for food.[2] Some social animal species communicate the signal of potential danger by stopping contact calls, without the use of alarm calls. Charles Darwin wrote about this in relation to wild horse and cattle.[3]

Humming as contact calls

Joseph Jordania suggested that human humming could have played a function of contact calls in early human ancestors.[4] According to his suggestion, humans find it distressing to be in full silence (which is a sign of danger for them), and this is why humans who are alone sometimes hum, whistle, talk to themselves, or listen to TV or radio during other activities.

Birds

Birds use contact calls in flight to establish location and to keep aware of each other's presence while flying and feeding.[5] For some species, this call consists of a short, high-pitched sound, recognized and duplicated exactly by mates. Some fowl, such as geese,"honk" while in migration to communicate location and proximity to others in their flock.[6]

In parapatric and sympatric species of birds, where territories of one species may border or overlap with that of another species, the contact calls of each species may divergently evolve to help differentiate one species from that of the other within close proximity.[7] Where one species may have little to no variation among the contact calls of distinct allopatric populations, populations with overlapping territories may change their contact call to sound different from the bordering population's. The same variation pattern is seen in certain species of birds, such as the White-eyed Birds (Zosterops spp.) with increasing elevation and longitude/latitude.[7]

Warning

Some species of birds have alarm calls to specifically warn other individuals of predators. The Black-capped Chickadee warns its kind of the level of threat an approaching predator is by the number of "de"s heard. Its call, chick-a-dee-dee-dee, might indicate more danger than chick-a-dee-dee. Some calls reveal more details about an approaching predator, indicated by the pitch or speed. When there is a threatening enemy in the air, such as a hawk or eagle, the Florida Scrub-Jay warns other jays to seek cover by using a thin, shrill-like call. In contrast, an approaching predatory feline provokes a low-pitched "scolding" sound, and calls on fellow jays for help in scaring the intruder away.[6]

Comfort

Parrots kept as pets demonstrate contact calls with their human owners. Parrots make their call to establish that the human is within earshot, and continue to make the call (sometimes growing louder into a scream) until acknowledged. The screaming develops in pet parrots, as well as wild flock, when the animal feels like its needs are not being met because the contact call is not being understood.[8]

Deer

Deer communicate with contact calls, alarm calls and other calls to signal when they are ready to breed, to feed, to locate others in their group, after they have shed their velvet, to signal frustration, to summon nearby deer and to intimidate unwanted visitors.

Non-aggressive

  • Location: Main contact call to signify location.
  • Doe Grunt: Doe's call; used to summon fawns for feeding.
  • Buck Grunt: Buck's call; used to summon fawns.
  • Sparring: Signals that the deer has shed its velvet.
  • Breeding Bellow: Doe is ready to breed.

Aggressive

  • Sniff: Deer uses this call to intimidate unwanted deer visitors and prevent fighting.
  • Wheeze: Buck makes this sound to intimidate other deer and to prevent fights.

Primates

A troop of Ring-Tailed Lemurs

Ring-Tailed Lemurs

A ring-tailed lemur's contact call is used heavily to communicate and is classified on the level of excitement the lemur achieves in a situation. When low to moderately excited, the lemur moans; a "meow" is heard when the excitement is raised to a moderate level or when a situation will bring group togetherness. The loudest call occurs when a lemur is separated from its troop, and is recognizable by a distinct "wail."[9]

Pygmy Marmosets

Pygmy marmosets have developed a vocal system in which two acoustically different contact calls have been established.[10] These two different calls allow individual marmosets to identify others within their population. The call an individual uses varies as a response to the call that they've heard. If one of the contact calls is played over a loudspeaker (from a familiar location to the marmoset), the response from a given individual can be predicted, indicating that the calls are used as an identification mechanism of communication. [10]

Baboons

Chacma Baboons have been observed to use contact calls not only as identification tools and locators of members of the group, but also as a way of communicating messages with one another about their status with respect to the main group. Mother baboons can recognize and locate the contact calls or barks of her offspring when they venture to forage or explore independently, such as to be able to find them if they get lost and need help finding their way back. The mother never barks back, unless she is in danger of being separated from the group (i.e. searching for her offspring will lead her too far astray from the group as it moves across the terrain). In this case, she will venture out slightly in the direction of the calling infant and bark back intermittently to let the lost baboon track her voice down and make its way back to the group.[2]

See also

References

  1. tige Macedonia, J. (1986). Individuality in the contact call of the ring-tailed lemur (Lemur catta). American Journal of Primatology, 11, 163-179
  2. Rendall, Drew; Cheney, Dorothy L.; Seyfarth, Robert M. (2000). "Proximate factors mediating "contact" calls in adult female baboons (Papio cynocephalus ursinus) and their infants". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 114 (1): 36–46. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.114.1.36. PMID 10739310. S2CID 10179274.
  3. Charles Darwin (2004). The Descent of Men. London: Penguin Books. pg. 123.
  4. Jordania, J. (2009). Times to Fight and Times to Relax: Singing and Humming at the Beginnings of Human Evolutionary History. Kadmos, 1, 272–277
  5. "Contact Call Definition - Bird Contact Calls - Parrot Contact Calls". Birds.about.com. 2012-04-09. Retrieved 2012-07-05.
  6. "All About Birds : Why Birds Call". Birds.cornell.edu. Retrieved 2012-07-05.
  7. Husemann, Martin; Ulrich, Werner; Habel, Jan (2014). "The evolution of contact calls in isolated and overlapping populations of two white-eye congeners in East Africa (Aves, Zosterops)". BMC Evolutionary Biology. 14 (1): 115. doi:10.1186/1471-2148-14-115. ISSN 1471-2148. PMC 4057569. PMID 24885807.
  8. Hallander, Jane. "The Communication Link With Our Parrots". Archived from the original on October 18, 1999. Retrieved March 20, 2012.
  9. Lang KA, Cawthon. "Primate Factsheets: Ring-tailed lemur (Lemur catta) Behavior". Retrieved March 20, 2012.
  10. Snowdon, Charles T.; Cleveland, Jayne (1980). "Individual recognition of contact calls by pygmy marmosets". Animal Behaviour. 28 (3): 717–727. doi:10.1016/s0003-3472(80)80131-x. ISSN 0003-3472. S2CID 53186935.
This article is issued from Wikipedia. The text is licensed under Creative Commons - Attribution - Sharealike. Additional terms may apply for the media files.