Dora Biro
Dora Biro is a behavioral biologist and the Beverly Petterson Bishop and Charles W. Bishop Professor, Brain and Cognitive Sciences at the University of Rochester.[1][2] She was previously a Professor of Animal Behaviour at the University of Oxford.[3] and a visiting professor in the Primate Research Institute of Kyoto University in Japan.[4] Biro studies social behavior, problem solving, and learning in birds and primates.
Dora Biro | |
---|---|
Education | University of Oxford |
Scientific career | |
Institutions | University of Rochester, University of Oxford, Kyoto University |
External video | |
---|---|
“Dora Biro - Collective Action at the Cognition-culture Interface”, ASAB Summer, September 16, 2019. |
Early life and education
Biro earned her BA in Biological Sciences (1997) and DPhil in Animal Behaviour (2002) at the University of Oxford.[5]
Career
Biro was a postdoctoral research fellow with funding from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS) in 2002-2003. She held an Engineering and Physical Sciences Research Council (EPSRC) postdoctoral fellowship at the University of Oxford from 2003-2006. She was a visiting professor at the Primate Research Institute of Kyoto University, Japan in 2007.[4]
In 2007 Biro returned to the University of Oxford as a Royal Society University Research Fellow.[4] In 2013 she became an associate professor in animal behaviour in the department of zoology and a fellow of St Hugh's College, Oxford[6] and in 2019 a full professor in animal behaviour in the department of zoology.[4]
As of 2021 Biro joined the University of Rochester in Rochester, New York[7] and was appointed as the Beverly Petterson Bishop and Charles W. Bishop Professor of Brain and Cognitive Sciences.[1]
Research
Biro studies social behavior, problem solving, and learning in birds and primates, with particular attention to processes that support living in groups. She uses techniques such as field observations and technologies such as GPS tracking, accelerometry, camera trapping, artificial intelligence software for chimpanzee facial recognition,[1][8] and mathematical modeling.[1] She has also tried introducing robots to see if they can influence avian behavior.[9]
Biro studies the impact of individual differences in two major systems of behavior:[10] social learning and group decision-making in the navigation of homing pigeons,[11][12][13] and cultural learning among wild chimpanzees.[14]
Through a series of studies at Oxford, Biro has examined how homing pigeons change their navigation behavior and learn new routes. Birds prefer individual routes[3] and can remember them for years.[15] After 12 flights, pigeons tended to fix their route. While pigeons may gradually shorten their routes over time, they are more likely to do so after a new "naive" bird is paired with a bird that has flown the route before. The introduction of a newcomer increased the time spent on exploration and resulted in the collective learning of better routes.[3] Biro's work on bird navigation has presented evidence for cumulative cultural evolution, the ability to transfer knowledge across generations. This type of learning was previously thought to be too cognitively complex for birds, and possibly limited to humans.[11]
In addition, her work with at Kyoto University with wild chimpanzees has shown that younger inexperienced chimpanzees are more likely to engage in innovation (like cracking a new kind of nut) than older chimpanzees, and more likely to learn from each other.[3] Older chimpanzees were less likely to learn innovative behaviors from younger ones than from those of their own age or older. New behaviors were more likely to be learned by adults if they were introduced by an adult new to the group.[16]
In 2020, Biro and others reported the first observations of tool use in seabirds, after Atlantic puffins at breeding colonies in Wales and Iceland were observed spontaneously using small wooden sticks to scratch themselves.[17][18] In other work Biro has suggested that tool use is more likely to scaffold learning if the tools themselves are durable.[19]
References
- "Eight faculty members appointed to named professorships | PressReleasePoint". Press Release Point. University of Rochester. February 15, 2022. Retrieved 10 August 2022.
- "Dora Biro". University of Rochester. Retrieved 10 August 2022.
- Vernimmen, Tim (29 July 2022). "Cultural transmission makes animals flexible, but vulnerable". Knowable Magazine | Annual Reviews. doi:10.1146/knowable-072822-1. Retrieved 10 August 2022.
- "Dora Biro". Center for Academic Research and Training in Anthropogeny (CARTA). Retrieved 10 August 2022.
- "Dora Biro". Oxford Mosaic. Retrieved 11 August 2022.
- "The principal and fellows of St. Hugh's College in the University of Oxford". Charity Commission. Retrieved 11 August 2022.
- "New Faculty 2021-2022". University of Rochester. Retrieved 11 August 2022.
- "Artificial intelligence used to recognise primate faces in the wild". www.biology.ox.ac.uk. 4 September 2019. Retrieved 11 August 2022.
- Holmes, Bob (November 8, 2021). "How Scientists Are Using Robotic Animals to Learn About Real Ones". Smithsonian Magazine. Retrieved 11 August 2022.
- "Professor Dora Biro". University of Oxford. Retrieved 11 August 2022.
- Bodkin, Henry (18 April 2017). "Bird brains not so stupid: pigeons show human-like ability to build knowledge through generations". The Telegraph. Retrieved 11 August 2022.
- Gorman, James (26 September 2016). "Pigeons Resist Misguided Leaders". The New York Times. Retrieved 11 August 2022.
- Weaver, Janelle (7 April 2010). "Airborne pigeons obey the pecking order". Nature. doi:10.1038/news.2010.168. Retrieved 11 August 2022.
- Schofield, Daniel; Nagrani, Arsha; Zisserman, Andrew; Hayashi, Misato; Matsuzawa, Tetsuro; Biro, Dora; Carvalho, Susana (6 September 2019). "Chimpanzee face recognition from videos in the wild using deep learning". Science Advances. 5 (9): eaaw0736. Bibcode:2019SciA....5..736S. doi:10.1126/sciadv.aaw0736. ISSN 2375-2548. PMC 6726454. PMID 31517043.
- Donovan, Robin (March 1, 2022). "Homing Pigeons Remember Routes for Years". Retrieved 11 August 2022.
- Biro, Dora; Inoue-Nakamura, Noriko; Tonooka, Rikako; Yamakoshi, Gen; Sousa, Claudia; Matsuzawa, Tetsuro (December 2003). "Cultural innovation and transmission of tool use in wild chimpanzees: evidence from field experiments". Animal Cognition. 6 (4): 213–223. doi:10.1007/s10071-003-0183-x. ISSN 1435-9448. PMID 12898285. S2CID 651756. Retrieved 11 August 2022.
- Yirka, Bob (January 3, 2020). "First evidence found of tool use by seabirds". phys.org. Retrieved 11 August 2022.
- Fayet, Annette L.; Hansen, Erpur Snær; Biro, Dora (21 January 2020). "Evidence of tool use in a seabird". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 117 (3): 1277–1279. Bibcode:2020PNAS..117.1277F. doi:10.1073/pnas.1918060117. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 6983420. PMID 31889002.
- Flurry, Alan (15 October 2013). "New study shows use of tools supports learning in nonhuman species". UGA Today. Retrieved 11 August 2022.