Machiavellianism (psychology)

In the field of personality psychology, Machiavellianism is a personality trait characterized by interpersonal manipulation, indifference to morality, lack of empathy, and a strategic focus on self-interest.[3][4][5][6] Psychologists Richard Christie and Florence Geis named the trait after Niccolò Machiavelli, as they used edited and truncated statements inspired by his works to study variations in human behaviors.[7][8][9][10] Their Mach IV test, a 20-question, Likert-scale personality survey, became the standard self-assessment tool and scale of the Machiavellianism construct. Those who score high on the scale (High Machs) are more likely to have a high level of deceitfulness and a cynical, unemotional temperament.[11][12]

Machiavellianism
Machiavellianism is one of the traits in the dark triad model, along with psychopathy and narcissism.
SpecialtyPersonality psychology
CausesGenetic and environmental[1][2]

It is one of the dark triad traits, along with the subclinical versions of narcissism and psychopathy.[13][14][15]

Core features

In developing the construct studying manipulators, Christie theorized that they would possess the following characteristics:[16]

1. A relative lack of affect in interpersonal relationships: Manipulators do not empathize with their victims. The more empathy one has, Christie says, the less likely one will manipulate a person to do their bidding.

2. A lack of concern for conventional morality: Christie asserts that the manipulator is not concerned with the morality of behaviors such as lying and cheating.

3. A lack of gross psychopathology (mental illness): Christie states that manipulators usually have an instrumentalist view of the world, which shows a lack of psychosis or other mental impairments.

4. Low ideological commitment: Manipulators prefer to focus on getting things done pragmatically rather than focus on ideological allegiances. Christie states that while manipulators are to be found in organizations of differing ideals, they are more likely to be involved in tactics that achieve individual ends than inflexible idealistic ones.[17]

Five Factor Model

Under the recently devised Five-Factor Model of Machiavellianism, three characteristics underlie the construct:[18]

  • Antagonism: manipulativeness, cynicism, selfishness, callousness, and arrogance.
  • Planfulness: deliberation and orderliness.
  • Agency: achievement-striving, assertiveness, self-confidence, emotional invulnerability, activity and competence.

Origin of the construct

Richard Christie was inspired by the works of Niccolò Machiavelli in studying the behaviors of those who manipulate others.

Inspiration from Machiavelli

In the early 16th century, Niccolò Machiavelli wrote several works on politics, including The Prince and The Discourses on Livy. These works were to bring Machiavelli everlasting fame, as they provided controversial statements regarding how monarchs should conduct themselves, such as deceiving enemies, and breaking pacts.[19] In the 1960s, Richard Christie and Florence L. Geis wanted to study the thought processes and actions of those who manipulate others, such as political ideologues and religious extremists. Christie was inspired by Machiavelli's writings, so he and his colleagues developed a test using a selection of statements, including truncated and edited sentences from Machiavelli's works as test items, naming the construct "Machiavellianism" after him.[20][4] They wanted to assess whether or not those who were in agreement with the statements would behave differently than others who disagreed, specifically in regards to manipulative actions. They published their results in their book, titled "Studies in Machiavellianism".

Though Christie named the trait after Machiavelli, he makes clear that he used sentences from Machiavelli's works only as a sort of litmus test to study deceptive and manipulative behavior, and that he is not concerned with their historical or philosophical significance, stating specifically that:[21]

Historians disagree as to whether Machiavelli was a cynic who wrote political satire, a patriot, or the first modern political scientist. The present concern is not with Machiavelli as an historic figure, but as the source of ideas about those who manipulate others.

Mach IV

Their Mach IV test, a 20-question, Likert-scale personality survey, became the standard self-assessment tool of the Machiavellianism construct.[8] Using their scale, Christie and Geis conducted multiple experimental tests that showed that the interpersonal strategies and behavior of "high Machs" and "low Machs" differ.[22] Those who score highly on the scale are classified as high Machs, while those who score low are classified as low Machs. People scoring high on the scale (high Machs) tend to endorse manipulative statements, and behave accordingly, contrary to those who score lowly (low Machs).[23] People scoring high on the scale (high Machs) tend to endorse statements such as, "Never tell anyone the real reason you did something unless it is useful to do so," (No. 1) but not ones like, "Most people are basically good and kind" (No. 4), "There is no excuse for lying to someone else" (No. 7) or "Most people who get ahead in the world lead clean, moral lives" (No. 11)[24] Their basic results have been widely replicated.[25] Measured on the Mach IV scale, males score, on average, slightly higher on Machiavellianism than females.[22][26]

The Mach IV test influenced the creation of assessment called the Dirty Dozen, which contains 12 items, and the Short Dark Triad, composed of 27 items.[27]

Causes

Genetic and environmental

Several behavioral genetics studies on the dark triad have shown that Machiavellianism has both significantly genetic and environmental influences.[28][29][30][31] One of the studies noted that while Machiavellianism is heritable to a substantial degree, it can also be influenced by the shared-environment (i.e. sibling groups) slightly more than narcissism and psychopathy.[32][33][34] Other traits associated with machiavellianism are influenced by genetics as well, as one study notes that "The co-occurrence of alexithymia and Machiavellianism was most heavily influenced by genetic factors, and to a lesser but significant extent by non-shared environmental factors."[35] Machiavellianism is also heavily correlated with primary psychopathy which is itself strongly heritable.[36] A study on the "core" of dark triad traits also emphasized that the residual traits of Machiavellianism had "significant genetic components".[2] One particular study found a gene responsible for dopamine reception was positively associated with individuals who scored high on the MACH IV, but it is unclear what specific mechanisms cause this effect.[37][38]:92

The environmental causes (such as shared, and non-shared environment) that contribute to the development of machiavellianism were childhood maltreatment and neglect, social reinforcement of manipulative behaviors from an early age, and poor family functioning.[32][39] One study even stated that "the etiology of Machiavellianism, similarly to the development of a dismissing-avoidant pattern, partly originates from childhood experiences obtained in relationships with unexpressive, less understanding, highly punitive or restrictive caregivers".[40][41]

Machiavellianism in children

Ever since the creation of the construct in the 1960s, there has been extensive research on Machiavellianism in young children and adolescents, via a measure dubbed the "Kiddie Mach" test.[42][43] The first study was done in 1966 as a part of Dorothea Braginsky's doctoral dissertation, with the subjects being as young as 10 years old.[44] Studies have shown that traits of Machiavellianism and other dark triad traits were already present in adolescents aged 11–17.[45] Deceptive behaviors by children even as young as 3 were also investigated heavily.[46][47][48] Peer reports suggest that children higher in Machiavellianism exhibit behaviors such as using both prosocial and coercive strategies based on how much is to be gained in a situation, and they tend to manipulate indirectly.[49] Children who score highly on the Machiavellianism scale tend to be more successful in manipulation, do it more frequently, and are judged as better at manipulation than those who score lower.[50] Parental levels of Machiavellianism seem to have a slight effect on the child's own level. Machiavellianism levels in fathers was positively correlated with the Machiavellianism levels of their children, but the mother's level had no significant effect. One study concluded that "parental Machiavellianism is a predictor and perhaps a cause of children's Machiavellian beliefs and their manipulative success".[51] Machiavellianism is also correlated with childhood aggression, especially concerning the control of social hierarchies.[52] One study found a trend upwards with respect to Machiavellianism from late childhood to adolescence, when levels of Machiavellianism are thought to peak. From adolescence throughout adulthood there is a significant and steady downward trend with regard to levels of Machiavellianism, until the age of 65 where an overall lifetime minimum is reached.[53]

Peer ratings of Machiavellian children are inconsistent, with some researchers reporting that Machiavellian children are rated as popular,[54] and some reporting that they are less well liked by peers.[55][56]

Dark Triad

In 1998, John McHoskey, William Worzel, and Christopher Szyarto proposed that narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy are more or less interchangeable in normal samples.[57][58] Delroy L. Paulhus and McHoskey debated these perspectives at an American Psychological Association conference, inspiring a body of research that continues to grow in the published literature. Delroy Paulhus and Kevin Williams found enough differences between the traits to suggest that they were distinct despite their similarities, thus the concept of a "triad" of offensive personality traits was conceptualized.[59] There has been research on Machiavellianism using various dark triad measures, including the Short Dark Triad (SD3), and the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen test.[60][61]

Psychopathy

Many psychologists consider Machiavellianism to be essentially indistinguishable from psychopathy, as they both share manipulative tendencies and cold callousness as their primary attributes.[62][63][64] According to John McHoskey, the MACH-IV test is merely "a global measure of psychopathy in noninstitutionalized populations".[65] Both psychopaths and Machiavellians score low on conscientiousness and agreeableness, and they often are dismissive of social norms and ethics.[66] On the Psychopathic Personality Inventory, created by Scott Lilienfeld and Brian Andrews, there is even a subscale similarly named "Machiavellian Egocentricity".[67][68][69] The subscale is said to assess "ruthless attitudes in interpersonal functioning".[70]

Many other psychologists state that while Machiavellianism and psychopathy overlap heavily, there is much evidence to suggest that they are distinct personality constructs.[71][72][73] Psychologists who stress the differences between Machiavellianism and psychopathy state that psychopaths differ from individuals high in Machiavellianism in that they are impulsive, tend to be reckless, and they lack long term planning skills.[74][75][76] Delroy Paulhus and others have stated that this difference between the two traits is often underappreciated.[77] High Machs have been described as "master manipulators" and far better at manipulation than psychopaths and narcissists.[78][79][80][81]

Narcissism

Individuals high in Machiavellianism and narcissism both manipulate to improve their reputations, and how they appear to others.[82][83] Individuals high in the two traits do this as a form of self aggrandizement to help their chances of success in a given situation.[84] Machiavellianism scores were positively associated with aspects of narcissism such as entitlement and exploitativeness, and inversely associated with adaptive narcissistic tendencies, like self-sufficiency.[85] Studies have also shown that Machiavellians are more realistic about their character, while narcissists are less realistic about theirs.[86] Compared to High Machs, narcissists are less malevolent and show a more socially positive personality. They also have higher levels of self-rated happiness.[87]

White collar crime

Research has shown that individuals high in Machiavellianism may be more willing to engage in white collar crimes.[88][89][90] Delroy Paulhus has stated that Machiavellianism is the main trait for con artists, and not psychopathy, stating that:

Although direct research on this topic is difficult, it seems clear that malevolent stockbrokers such as Bernie Madoff do not qualify as psychopaths: They are corporate Machiavellians who use deliberate, strategic procedures for exploiting others. A genuine psychopath, even at the subclinical level, lacks the self-control to orchestrate the schemes of a shrewd stockbroker.[91]

DSM: Trait, not disorder

Machiavellianism has never been considered a disorder, nor has it been referenced in any version of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders.[92][93] It has been treated as strictly a personality construct. It is primarily studied by personality psychologists, as it is a non-clinical personality style.[94]

Relations with other personality traits

Big Five

Mach-IV scores are negatively correlated with agreeableness (r = −0.47) and conscientiousness (r = −0.34), two dimensions of the "big five" personality model (NEO-PI-R).[75] The FFMI corrects for this by including aspects of high conscientiousness in the scale (e.g. order, deliberation).[18] Additionally, Machiavellianism correlates more highly with the honesty-humility dimension of the six-factor HEXACO model than with any of the big five dimensions.[71] Machiavellianism has also been located within the interpersonal circumplex, which consists of the two independent dimensions of agency and communion.[95][96][97] Agency refers to the motivation to succeed and to individuate the self, whereas communion refers to the motivation to merge with others and to support group interests.[98] Machiavellianism lies in the quadrant of the circumplex defined by high agency and low communion.[71] Machiavellianism has been found to lie diagonally opposite from a circumplex construct called self-construal, a tendency to prefer communion over agency. This suggests that people high in Machiavellianism do not simply wish to achieve, they wish to do so at the expense of (or at least without regard to) others.[71][99]

Hot and cold empathy

There are two distinct types of empathy which people use to relate to each other which are referred to as hot and cold empathy. Cold empathy refers to the understanding of how others might react to one's actions or a certain event. Hot empathy refers to the emotional reaction others might have to the emotions of another person. People high in Machiavellianism tend to have a better understanding of cold empathy and do not feel hot empathy which explains why they seem cold and uncaring.[100][101] Some studies have suggested that Machiavellians are deficient only at the level of affective empathy (sharing of emotions), whereas their cognitive empathy is intact, even high.[102][103][104][105] Another study suggested that high Machs are deficient at both kinds of empathy.[106] Studies also assert that high Machs don't feel guilt over the consequences of their manipulations.[107] High Machs are less likely to be altruistic, and they are less likely to be concerned with the problems of others.[108]

Alexithymia

Alexithymia is considered a key trait correlated heavily with Machiavellianism.[109][102][110] It is the lack of awareness of one's own emotions as well as the emotions of others.[111] When tested, healthy alexithymic individuals have been found to obtain high Machiavellianism scores.[112][102] This was not surprising to researchers, seeing as one of the core traits of Machiavellianism is unemotionality, similar to what alexithymics experience.[113]

Motivation

A 1992 review described the motivation of those high on the Machiavellianism scale as related to cold selfishness and pure instrumentality, and those high on the trait were assumed to pursue their motives (e.g. sex, achievement, sociality) in duplicitous ways. More recent research on the motivations of high Machs compared to low Machs found that they gave high priority to money, power, and competition and relatively low priority to community building, self-love, and family commitment. High Machs admitted to focusing on unmitigated achievement and winning at any cost.[71][114][115]:93

The research on behaviors which high Machs engage in suggest that they are willing to achieve their goals by bending and breaking rules, cheating, and stealing.[116] People high in Machiavellianism are able to easily switch between working with others to taking advantage of others to achieve their goals, and they are more willing to do things others see as terrible or immoral.[100][117][118]

Mental abilities

Due to their skill at interpersonal manipulation, there has often been an assumption that high Machs possess superior intelligence, or ability to understand other people in social situations. Recent research provides some support for this assumption.[119] However, other research has established that Machiavellianism is unrelated to IQ.[120][121]

Furthermore, studies on emotional intelligence have found that high Machiavellianism is usually associated with low emotional intelligence as assessed by both performance and questionnaire measures.[122] Both emotional empathy and emotion recognition have been shown to have negative correlations with Machiavellianism.[123][124] Additionally, research has shown that Machiavellianism is unrelated to a more advanced theory of mind, that is, the ability to anticipate what others are thinking in social situations.[125] However, some studies have suggested the contrary viewpoint that high Machiavellianism is associated with excellent theory of mind skills.[126][127][128][102]

When it comes to manipulation, individuals high in Machiavellianism may, according to Bereczkei, "have certain cognitive and social skills that enable them to properly adapt to the challenges of environmental circumstances".[129] They also are incredibly perceptive to the presence of others, and feign altruism to enhance their reputation.[130]

Social relationships

The effects that one's level of Machiavellianism has on a person's socialization and interpersonal relationships, such as friendships and romantic relationships, has been studied extensively. High Machs are more than likely to choose better quality friends, as they can guess better who is a good person and thus more pliable for manipulation.[131] Machiavellianism was also correlated with withdrawal and avoidance in romantic relationships.[132] Individuals high in all dark triad traits find it easy to end relationships, and tend to prefer short-term relationships over long term ones.[133] Women higher on machiavellianism tend to go on dates not for sexual reasons but for free food, a phenomenon known as a "foodie call".[134] Because a lack of empathy and affect with regards to others is one of the main features of Machiavellianism, individuals high on the trait tend to act in a utilitarian, self interested manner, prefer emotionally detached relationships, and are not concerned with the other person's needs.[135][136] High machs report lower relationship satisfaction than those lower on the scale.[137] Though there has been research on the potential "attractiveness" of the dark triad traits,[138] out of all of the traits in the dark triad, Machiavellianism was the least attractive to the opposite sex.[139][140][141] One of the studies concluded that "The third DT trait, Machiavellianism, was significantly negatively associated with being chosen and mate appeal for STR [short term relationships] in women."[140] Another study claimed that this was because high Machs tend to be way less extroverted than narcissists and psychopaths, and that "it is possible that individuals do not like cynical, manipulative, aggressive, remorseless, and duplicitous people such as Machiavellians and psychopaths".[141]

In the workplace

Machiavellianism is also studied by organizational psychologists, especially those who study manipulative behaviors in workplace settings.[142][143][144] Workplace behaviors associated with this concept include flattery, deceit, coercion, and the abuse of others through one's position of leadership.[145][146][147] These behaviors in the workplace are ultimately done to advance personal interests.[148][149][83] Research has shown that one's level of Machiavellianism can be a major factor in situations where workplace manipulation is involved because this trait can have an effect on the ability for an individual to "fit" into a highly political work environment.[150] Research has found individuals with Dark Triad traits are drawn to entrepreneurship. Certain qualities found in the Dark Triad are similar to traits needed for effective entrepreneurship, such as confidence, charisma and risk taking.[151][152]

Career advancement

Individuals high in machiavellianism tend to gravitate towards particular careers, especially those that require a high degree of competitiveness needed to succeed.[153] High Machs are ambitious enough to cut corners and use aggressive means if it is necessary to get ahead in their careers.[153] One study found that "Machiavellianism was positively related to leadership position and career satisfaction".[154] Individuals high in machiavellian traits are especially drawn to leadership and management positions, which became an important subject in the primary literature. Sales careers also attract dark triad individuals, with one study stating that they are "prevalent" in the industry.[155] One study noted that those who possess machiavellian traits "are more productive but received lower overall managerial ratings", and that "Machiavellianism may in certain circumstances, be somewhat advantageous for long-term sales performance."[156] Machiavellianism was also associated with the use of "hard" (i.e. aggressive and hostile behavior) and "soft" (i.e. joking/kidding, offering compliments) tactics in the workforce.[157]

Dimensions of the MACH scale

Although there have been a myriad of proposed factor structures, two dimensions emerge most consistently within factor-analytic research – differentiating Machiavellian views from behaviors.[158] Although many posit that the Mach IV scale is unable to reliably capture the two dimensions, a 10-item subset of the scale known as the "two-dimensional Mach IV" (TDM-V), reproduces the views and tactics dimensions across countries, genders, sample types, and scale category length.[159][160] The "Views" dimension appears to capture the neurotic, narcissistic, pessimistic, and distrustful aspects of Machiavellianism, while the "Tactics" component captures the more unconscientious, self-serving, and deceitful behavioral aspects. More recently, in response to criticisms of the Mach-IV, researchers developed the Five-Factor Machiavellianism Inventory (FFMI), which attempts to include concepts (like being calculated and planful) that are not adequately captured by the Mach-IV.[18]

Scale Evaluation

Psychologist John Rauthmann and others have stated that, while the MACH-IV is "a generally reliable and valid scale", it has its shortcomings. These include the response styles of the test takers, the varying factor structures, and " insufficient content and construct validity". The researchers developed their own scale instead to study Machiavellianism multidimensionally instead of unidimensionally to prevent the construct from becoming hard to study effectively.[161] Psychologist Jason Dahling and others have created another measure of Machiavellianism, dubbed the Machiavellian Personality Scale (or MPS for short).[162]

Game theory

In 2002, the Machiavellianism scale of Christie and Geis was applied by behavioral game theorists Anna Gunnthorsdottir, Kevin McCabe and Vernon L. Smith in their search for explanations for the spread of observed behavior in experimental games, in particular individual choices which do not correspond to assumptions of material self-interest captured by the standard Nash equilibrium prediction.[26] It was found that in a trust game, those with high Mach-IV scores tended to follow Homo economicus' equilibrium strategies while those with low Mach-IV scores tended to deviate from the equilibrium, and instead made choices that reflected widely accepted moral standards and social preferences.[163]

A study done by David Wilson and other researchers noted that while High Machs tend to defect from their groups, they are also unlikely to succeed in the long term simply by manipulating others, and that some cooperation is necessary for further success and to avoid a situation in which they are retaliated against.[164][165]

See also

References

  1. Vernon, Philip A.; Villani, Vanessa C.; Vickers, Leanne C.; Harris, Julie Aitken (2008). "A behavioral genetic investigation of the Dark Triad and the Big 5". Personality and Individual Differences. 44 (2): 445–452. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.09.007.
  2. Schermer, Julie Aitken; Jones, Daniel N. (2020). "The behavioral genetics of the dark triad core versus unique trait components: A pilot study". Personality and Individual Differences. 154: 109701. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2019.109701. S2CID 210501437.
  3. "APA Dictionary of Psychology".
  4. Jones, D. N., & Paulhus, D. L. (2009). Machiavellianism. In M. R. Leary & R. H. Hoyle (Eds.), Handbook of individual differences in social behavior (pp. 93–108). New York: The Guilford Press.
  5. Christie, Richard; Geis, Florence L. (2013-10-22). Studies in Machiavellianism. Academic Press. ISBN 978-1-4832-6060-0.
  6. "Machiavellianism | Definition, Politics, Psychology, & Facts | Britannica". www.britannica.com. 2023-05-02. Retrieved 2023-06-21.
  7. "Since most of the scale items and some of our research notions came from The Prince and The Discourses, it seems only candid to give credit where credit is due. Perhaps this book can be viewed as a partial vindication of Machiavelli's astuteness."
    -Studies in Machiavellianism, page 339
  8. Colman, Andrew M. (2015). A Dictionary of Psychology - Andrew M. Colman - Google Books. Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-965768-1.
  9. Studies in Machiavellianism, "Scale Construction", p. 10
  10. Rauthmann, J. F., & Will, T. (2011). "Proposing a multidimensional Machiavellianism conceptualization". Social Behavior and Personality, 39(3), 391–404.
  11. Spielberger, Charles D.; Butcher, James N. (2013). Advances in Personality Assessment. Routledge. ISBN 9781317844006.
  12. Geis, F. L. (1978). “Machiavellianism,” in Dimensions of Personality, eds H. London and J. E. Exner Jr. (New York, NY: Wiley), 305–363
  13. Paulhus, Delroy L; Williams, Kevin M (December 1, 2002). "The Dark Triad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy". Journal of Research in Personality. 36 (6): 556–563. doi:10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00505-6. S2CID 6535576 via ScienceDirect.
  14. Lyons, Minna (2019). The Dark Triad of Personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and Psychopathy in Everyday Life. Academic Press. ISBN 978-0-12-814292-9.
  15. Furnham, Adrian; Richards, Steven C.; Paulhus, Delroy L. (March 10, 2013). "The Dark Triad of Personality: A 10 Year Review: Dark Triad of Personality". Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 7 (3): 199–216. doi:10.1111/spc3.12018 via CrossRef.
  16. Christie, Richard; Geis, Florence L. (2013-10-22). Studies in Machiavellianism - Richard Christie, Florence L. Geis - Google Books. Academic Press. ISBN 9781483260600.
  17. Studies in Machiavellianism, pg 3.
  18. Collison, K. L., Vize, C. E., Miller, J. D., & Lynam, D. R. (2018). Development and preliminary validation of a five factor model measure of Machiavellianism. Psychological Assessment, 30(10), 1401–1407. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000637
  19. "Machiavellianism | Definition, Politics, Psychology, & Facts | Britannica". www.britannica.com. 2023-05-02. Retrieved 2023-06-23.
  20. Christie, Richard "On being detached about Machiavellianism"
  21. Studies in Machiavellianism, pg. 1
  22. Christie, R. & Geis, F. (1970) Studies in Machiavellianism. New York: Academic Press. p. 260
  23. "Meet the Machiavellians | Psychology Today". www.psychologytoday.com. Retrieved 2022-09-14.
  24. Christie, R., & Geis, F. L. (2013). Studies in machiavellianism. Academic Press.
  25. Repacholi, Betty; Slaughter, Virginia, eds. (March 2004). "Bypassing Empathy: A Machiavellian Theory of Mind and Sneaky Power". Individual Differences in Theory of Mind: Implications for Typical and Atypical Development. Psychology Press. pp. 40–67. doi:10.4324/9780203488508-7. ISBN 978-1-135-43234-8.
  26. Gunnthorsdottir, Anna; McCabe, Kevin; Smith, Vernon (2002). "Using the Machiavellianism instrument to predict trustworthiness in a bargaining game". Journal of Economic Psychology. 23: 49–66. doi:10.1016/S0167-4870(01)00067-8.
  27. Muris, Peter; Merckelbach, Harald; Otgaar, Henry; Meijer, Ewout (March 2017). "The Malevolent Side of Human Nature: A Meta-Analysis and Critical Review of the Literature on the Dark Triad (Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and Psychopathy)". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 12 (2): 183–204. doi:10.1177/1745691616666070. ISSN 1745-6916. PMID 28346115. S2CID 24809359.
  28. Petrides, K. V.; Vernon, Philip A.; Schermer, Julie Aitken; Veselka, Livia (2011). "Trait Emotional Intelligence and the Dark Triad Traits of Personality". Twin Research and Human Genetics. 14 (1): 35–41
  29. Vernon, Philip A.; Villani, Vanessa C.; Vickers, Leanne C.; Harris, Julie Aitken (2008). "A behavioral genetic investigation of the Dark Triad and the Big 5". Personality and Individual Differences. 44 (2): 445–452. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.09.007.
  30. Furnham, Adrian; Richards, Steven C.; Paulhus, Delroy L. (March 2013). "The Dark Triad of Personality: A 10 Year Review". Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 7 (3): 199–216. doi:10.1111/spc3.12018. S2CID 32281294.
  31. Veselka, L., Schermer, J. A., & Vernon, P. A. (2011). Beyond the big five: The dark triad and the supernumerary personality inventory. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 14(2), 158-168.
  32. Monaghan, Conal, Boris Bizumic, and Martin Sellbom. "Nomological network of two-dimensional Machiavellianism." Personality and Individual Differences 130 (2018): 161-173.
  33. Petrides, K. V., Vernon, P. A., Schermer, J. A., & Veselka, L. (2011). Trait emotional intelligence and the dark triad traits of personality. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 14(1), 35-41.
  34. "What Makes Someone a Master Manipulator? | Psychology Today". www.psychologytoday.com. Retrieved 2023-06-24.
  35. Cairncross, Molly; Veselka, Livia; Schermer, Julie Aitken; Vernon, Philip A. (June 2013). "A Behavioral Genetic Analysis of Alexithymia and the Dark Triad Traits of Personality". Twin Research and Human Genetics. 16 (3): 690–697. doi:10.1017/thg.2013.19. PMID 23561050. S2CID 40361747.
  36. Bereczkei, Tamas (2015). "The manipulative skill: Cognitive devices and their neural correlates underlying Machiavellian's decision making". Brain and Cognition. 99: 24–31. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2015.06.007. PMID 26189112. S2CID 2671039.
  37. "APA PsycNet".
  38. Bereczkei, Tamás (12 December 2017). Machiavellianism: The Psychology of Manipulation. Routledge. ISBN 9781351605588.
  39. Láng, A. (2015). The relation between memories of childhood psychological maltreatment and Machiavellianism. Personality and Individual Differences, 77, 81-85.
  40. Ináncsi T, Láng A, Bereczkei T. Machiavellianism and Adult Attachment in General Interpersonal Relationships and Close Relationships. Eur J Psychol. 2015 Feb 27;11(1):139-54. doi: 10.5964/ejop.v11i1.801. PMID 27247647; PMCID: PMC4873099.
  41. Jonason P. K., Lyons M., Bethell E. (2014). The making of Darth Vader: Parent-child care and the Dark Triad. Personality and Individual Differences, 67, 30–34. 10.1016/j.paid.2013.10.006
  42. Christie, Richard; Geis, Florence L. (2013). Studies in Machiavellianism. Academic Press. p. 331. ISBN 9781483260600.
  43. Chabrol, Henri; Van Leeuwen, Nikki; Rodgers, Rachel; Séjourné, Natalène (November 2009). "Contributions of psychopathic, narcissistic, Machiavellian, and sadistic personality traits to juvenile delinquency". Personality and Individual Differences. 47 (7): 734–739. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.06.020.
  44. Studies in Machiavellianism, page 331
  45. Lau, K. S. L., & Marsee, M. A. (2013). Exploring narcissism, psychopathy, and Machiavellianism in youth: Examination of associations with antisocial behavior and aggression. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 22(3), 355–367. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-012-9586-0
  46. "APA PsycNet". psycnet.apa.org. Retrieved 2023-06-26.
  47. Lee, Kang (June 2013). "Little Liars: Development of Verbal Deception in Children". Child Development Perspectives. 7 (2): 91–96. doi:10.1111/cdep.12023. PMC 3653594. PMID 23687515.
  48. Mitchell, Robert W.; Thompson, Nicholas S. (1986-01-01). Deception: Perspectives on Human and Nonhuman Deceit. State University of New York Press. ISBN 978-1-4384-1332-7.
  49. Abell, Loren; Qualter, Pamela; Brewer, Gayle; Barlow, Alexandra; Stylianou, Maria; Henzi, Peter; Barrett, Louise (2015-08-20). "Why Machiavellianism Matters in Childhood: The Relationship Between Children's Machiavellian Traits and Their Peer Interactions in a Natural Setting". Europe's Journal of Psychology. 11 (3): 484–493. doi:10.5964/ejop.v11i3.957. ISSN 1841-0413. PMC 4873058. PMID 27247672.
  50. Braginsky, Dorothea D. (1970). "Machiavellianism and manipulative interpersonal behavior in children". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 6: 77–99. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(70)90077-6.
  51. "APA PsycNet".
  52. Kerig, Patricia K.; Stellwagen, Kurt K. (2010). "Roles of Callous-Unemotional Traits, Narcissism, and Machiavellianism in Childhood Aggression". Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 32 (3): 343–352. doi:10.1007/s10862-009-9168-7. S2CID 145468978.
  53. Götz, Friedrich M.; Bleidorn, Wiebke; Rentfrow, Peter J. (October 2020). "Age differences in Machiavellianism across the life span: Evidence from a large‐scale cross‐sectional study". Journal of Personality. 88 (5): 978–992. doi:10.1111/jopy.12545. ISSN 0022-3506. PMID 32145085. S2CID 212629171.
  54. Hawley P. H. (2003). Prosocial and coercive configurations of resource control in early adolescence: A case for the well-adapted Machiavellian. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 49, 279–309. 10.1353/mpq.2003.0013
  55. Palmen, H. (2009). Friendships and aggression in elementary school: The friendships of aggressive children and the effects of having aggressive friends (Doctoral thesis, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands).
  56. Abell, L.; Qualter, P.; Brewer, G.; Barlow, A.; Stylianou, M.; Henzi, P.; Barrett, L. (2015). "Why Machiavellianism Matters in Childhood: The Relationship Between Children's Machiavellian Traits and Their Peer Interactions in a Natural Setting". Europe's Journal of Psychology. 11 (3): 484–493. doi:10.5964/ejop.v11i3.957. PMC 4873058. PMID 27247672.
  57. McHoskey, J. (1995). Narcissism and Machiavellianism. Psychological Reports, 77, 755–759.
  58. McHoskey, J. W., Worzel, W., & Szyarto, C. (1998). Machiavellianism and psychopathy. Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 74, 192–210.
  59. "The Light Triad vs. Dark Triad of Personality".
  60. Jonason, P. K.; Webster, G. D. (2010). "The dirty dozen: A concise measure of the dark triad". Psychological Assessment. 22 (2): 420–432. doi:10.1037/a0019265. PMID 20528068.
  61. Jones, Daniel N.; Paulhus, Delroy L. (February 2014). "Introducing the Short Dark Triad (SD3): A Brief Measure of Dark Personality Traits". Assessment. 21 (1): 28–41. doi:10.1177/1073191113514105. PMID 24322012. S2CID 17524487.
  62. Miller, Joshua D.; Hyatt, Courtland S.; Maples-Keller, Jessica L.; Carter, Nathan T.; Lynam, Donald R. (2017). "Psychopathy and Machiavellianism: A Distinction Without a Difference?". Journal of Personality. 85 (4): 439–453. doi:10.1111/jopy.12251. PMID 26971566.
  63. Lyons, Minna (2019). The Dark Triad of Personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and Psychopathy in Everyday Life. Academic Press. p. 44. ISBN 978-0-12-814292-9.
  64. Miller, Joshua D.; Vize, Colin; Crowe, Michael L.; Lynam, Donald R. (August 2019). "A Critical Appraisal of the Dark-Triad Literature and Suggestions for Moving Forward". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 28 (4): 353–360. doi:10.1177/0963721419838233. ISSN 0963-7214. S2CID 155545945.
  65. McHoskey, John W.; Worzel, William; Szyarto, Christopher (1998). "Machiavellianism and psychopathy". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 74 (1): 192–210. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.192. PMID 9457782.
  66. Hartley, D.,Master Manipulators and Psychopaths Psychology Today.
  67. "APA PsycNet". psycnet.apa.org. Retrieved 2023-05-31.
  68. Benning, S. D., Patrick, C. J., Hicks, B. M., Blonigen, D. M., & Krueger, R. F. (2003). Factor structure of the psychopathic personality inventory: validity and implications for clinical assessment. Psychological assessment, 15(3), 340.
  69. Kastner, Rebecca M., Martin Sellbom, and Scott O. Lilienfeld. "A comparison of the psychometric properties of the psychopathic personality inventory full-length and short-form versions." Psychological Assessment 24, no. 1 (2012): 261.
  70. Poythress, Norman G., John F. Edens, and Scott O. Lilienfeld. "Criterion-related validity of the Psychopathic Personality Inventory in a prison sample." Psychological assessment 10, no. 4 (1998): 426.
  71. Jones, Daniel N.; Paulhus, Delroy L. (2009). "Chapter 7. Machiavellianism". In Leary, Mark R.; Hoyle, Rick H (eds.). Handbook of Individual Differences in Social Behavior. New York/London: The Guilford Press. pp. 257–273. ISBN 978-1-59385-647-2.
  72. Paulhus, Delroy L.; Jones, Daniel N. (2015). "Measures of Dark Personalities". Measures of Personality and Social Psychological Constructs. pp. 562–594. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-386915-9.00020-6. ISBN 978-0-12-386915-9.
  73. Jones, Daniel N.; Mueller, Steven M. (2022-03-01). "Is Machiavellianism Dead or Dormant? The Perils of Researching a Secretive Construct". Journal of Business Ethics. 176 (3): 535–549. doi:10.1007/s10551-020-04708-w. ISSN 1573-0697. S2CID 231202731.
  74. "APA PsycNet". psycnet.apa.org. Retrieved 2022-09-14.
  75. Paulhus, Delroy L.; Williams, Kevin M. (2002). "The Dark Triad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy". Journal of Research in Personality. 36 (6): 556–563. doi:10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00505-6. S2CID 6535576.
  76. Bereczkei, Tamas "Machiavellianism: The Psychology of Manipulation" chap. 4
  77. Jones, Daniel N.; Paulhus, Delroy L. (February 2014). "Introducing the Short Dark Triad (SD3): A Brief Measure of Dark Personality Traits". Assessment. 21 (1): 28–41. doi:10.1177/1073191113514105. PMID 24322012. S2CID 17524487.
  78. Horowitz, Leonard M.; Strack, Stephen (2 November 2010). Handbook of Interpersonal Psychology: Theory, Research, Assessment, and Therapeutic Interventions. John Wiley & Sons. ISBN 9780470471609.
  79. Paulhus, Delroy L.; Jones, Daniel N. (2015). "Measures of Dark Personalities". Measures of Personality and Social Psychological Constructs. pp. 562–594. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-386915-9.00020-6. ISBN 9780123869159.
  80. Bereczkei, Tamás (12 December 2017). Machiavellianism: The Psychology of Manipulation. Routledge. ISBN 9781351605588.
  81. "Meet the Machiavellians | Psychology Today".
  82. Rauthmann, John F. (2011-09-01). "Acquisitive or protective self-presentation of dark personalities? Associations among the Dark Triad and self-monitoring". Personality and Individual Differences. Digit Ratio (2D:4D) and Individual Differences Research. 51 (4): 502–508. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.05.008. ISSN 0191-8869.
  83. Lyons, Minna (2019-01-11). The Dark Triad of Personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and Psychopathy in Everyday Life. Academic Press. ISBN 978-0-12-814292-9.
  84. Tracy, Jessica L.; Cheng, Joey T.; Robins, Richard W.; Trzesniewski, Kali H. (2009-04-01). "Authentic and Hubristic Pride: The Affective Core of Self-esteem and Narcissism". Self and Identity. 8 (2–3): 196–213. doi:10.1080/15298860802505053. ISSN 1529-8868. S2CID 18447793.
  85. McHoskey, J. "Narcissism and Machiavellianism
  86. Paulhus, D. L., Williams, K., & Harms, P. (2001). Shedding light on the dark triad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy. In Annual meeting of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology. San Antonio, TX.
  87. Egan, Vincent, Stephanie Chan, and Gillian W. Shorter. "The Dark Triad, happiness and subjective well-being." Personality and individual differences 67 (2014): 17-22.
  88. Konnikova, Maria (10 January 2017). The Confidence Game: Why We Fall for It . . . Every Time. Penguin. ISBN 9780143109877.
  89. Tang, Thomas Li‐Ping, Yuh‐Jia Chen, and Toto Sutarso. "Bad apples in bad (business) barrels: The love of money, Machiavellianism, risk tolerance, and unethical behavior." Management Decision 46.2 (2008): 243-263.
  90. Utami, I., Wijono, S., Noviyanti, S., & Mohamed, N. (2019). Fraud diamond, Machiavellianism and fraud intention. International Journal of Ethics and Systems.
  91. Paulhus, Delroy L. (2014). "Toward a Taxonomy of Dark Personalities". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 23 (6): 421–426. doi:10.1177/0963721414547737. S2CID 44987860.
  92. "Psychiatry.org - DSM History".
  93. "Machiavellianism | Definition, Politics, Psychology, & Facts | Britannica".
  94. "MACH has historically been the province of personality and social psychology" McHoskey, J. W., Worzel, W., & Szyarto, C. (1998). Machiavellianism and psychopathy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(1), 192–210.
  95. Rauthmann, John F., and Gerald P. Kolar. "Positioning the Dark Triad in the interpersonal circumplex: The friendly-dominant narcissist, hostile-submissive Machiavellian, and hostile-dominant psychopath?." Personality and Individual Differences 54.5 (2013): 622-627.
  96. McHoskey, John. "Narcissism and machiavellianism." Psychological reports 77.3 (1995): 755-759.
  97. Jones, Daniel N., and Delroy L. Paulhus. "Differentiating the Dark Triad within the interpersonal circumplex." Handbook of interpersonal psychology: Theory, research, assessment, and therapeutic interventions (2010): 249-267.
  98. Gurtman, Michael B. "Exploring personality with the interpersonal circumplex." Social and personality psychology compass 3.4 (2009): 601-619.
  99. Jones, Daniel N.; Paulhus, Delroy L. (2012), "Differentiating the Dark Triad Within the Interpersonal Circumplex", Handbook of Interpersonal Psychology, John Wiley & Sons, Ltd, pp. 249–267, doi:10.1002/9781118001868.ch15, ISBN 9781118001868, S2CID 55425895
  100. Taylor, Ben. Machiavellianism, Psych Central.
  101. Paal, Tunde, and Tamas Bereczkei. "Adult theory of mind, cooperation, Machiavellianism: The effect of mindreading on social relations." Personality and individual differences 43.3 (2007): 541-551.
  102. Al Aïn, Syrina; Carré, Arnaud; Fantini-Hauwel, Carole; Baudouin, Jean-Yves; Besche-Richard, Chrystel (2013). "What is the emotional core of the multidimensional Machiavellian personality trait?". Frontiers in Psychology. 4: 454. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00454. PMC 3717508. PMID 23885245. Text was copied from this source, which is available under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 (CC BY 3.0) license.
  103. Barnett, M. A., and Thompson, S. (1985). The role of perspective-taking and empathy in children's Machiavellianism, prosocial behaviour, and motive for helping. J. Genet. Psychol. 146, 295–305. doi:10.1080/00221325.1985.9914459
  104. Repacholi, B., Slaughter, V., Pritchard, M., and Gibbs, V. (2003). “Theory of mind, Machiavellism, and social functioning in childhood,” in Individual Differences in Theory of Mind. Macquarie Monographs in Cognitive Science, eds B. Repacholi, and V. Slaughter (Hove: Psychology Press), 99–120.
  105. Richell, R. A., Mitchell, D. G., Newman, C., Leonard, A., Baron-Cohen, S., and Blair, R. J. (2003). Theory of mind and psychopathy: can psychopathic individuals read the language of the eyes? Neuropsychologia 41, 523–526. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00175-6
  106. Lyons, M., Cadwell, T., and Shultz, S. (2010). Mind-reading and manipulation—is Machiavellianism related to theory of mind? J. Evol. Psychol. 8, 261–274. doi:10.1556/JEP.8.2010.3.7
  107. Leary, Mark R.; Hoyle, Rick H. (17 December 2013). Handbook of Individual Differences in Social Behavior. Guilford Publications. ISBN 9781462514892.
  108. Kaufman, Scott Barry. "The Light Triad vs. Dark Triad of Personality". Scientific American Blog Network.
  109. Wastell, Colin; Booth, Alexandra (December 2003). "Machiavellianism: An Alexithymic Perspective". Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 22 (6): 730–744. doi:10.1521/jscp.22.6.730.22931.
  110. "What Drives Master Manipulators | Psychology Today". www.psychologytoday.com. Retrieved 2022-09-15.
  111. "Alexithymia: Symptoms, diagnosis, and links with mental health". www.medicalnewstoday.com. 25 September 2019. Retrieved 2020-04-29.
  112. Simon, L. J., Francis, P. L., and Lombardo, J. P. (1990). Sex, sex-role, and Machiavellianism as correlates of decoding ability. Percept. Mot. Skills 71, 243–247. doi:10.2466/pms.1990.71.1.243
  113. Geis, F. L. (1978). “Machiavellianism,” in Dimensions of Personality, eds H. London and J. E. Exner Jr. (New York, NY: Wiley), 305–363.
  114. Spielberger, Charles D.; Butcher, James N. (2013). Advances in Personality Assessment. Routledge. ISBN 9781317844013.
  115. Christie, Richard; Geis, Florence L. (22 October 2013). Studies in Machiavellianism. Academic Press. ISBN 9781483260600.
  116. Leary, Mark R.; Hoyle, Rick H. (2013-12-17). Handbook of Individual Differences in Social Behavior. Guilford Publications. ISBN 978-1-4625-1489-2.
  117. "APA PsycNet".
  118. Rauthmann, John F. "Towards multifaceted Machiavellianism: Content, factorial, and construct validity of a German Machiavellianism Scale." Personality and Individual Differences 52.3 (2012): 345-351.
  119. Kowalski, Christopher Marcin; Kwiatkowska, Katarzyna; Kwiatkowska, Maria Magdalena; Ponikiewska, Klaudia; Rogoza, Radosław; Schermer, Julie Aitken (2018). "The Dark Triad traits and intelligence: Machiavellians are bright, and narcissists and psychopaths are ordinary". Personality and Individual Differences. 135: 1–6. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2018.06.049. S2CID 149807368.
  120. Jean M. Phillips; Stanley M. Gully (2013). Organizational Behavior: Tools for Success. Cengage Learning. pp. 85–. ISBN 978-1-133-95360-9.
  121. Machiavellianism: A synthesis of the evolutionary and psychological literatures. Wilson, David Sloan,Near, David,Miller, Ralph R. Psychological Bulletin, Vol 119(2), Mar 1996, 285-299
  122. Austin, Elizabeth J.; Farrelly, Daniel; Black, Carolyn; Moore, Helen (2007). "Emotional intelligence, Machiavellianism and emotional manipulation: Does EI have a dark side?". Personality and Individual Differences. 43: 179–189. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006.11.019.
  123. Christie, Richard; Geis, Florence L. (2013). Studies in Machiavellianism. Academic Press. ISBN 9781483260600.
  124. Rauthmann, John F.; Will, Theresa (30 April 2011). "Proposing a Multidimensional Machiavellianism Conceptualization". Social Behavior and Personality. 39 (3): 391–403. doi:10.2224/sbp.2011.39.3.391.
  125. Lyons, M.; Caldwell, T.; Shultz, S. (2010-09-01). "Mind-reading and manipulation — Is Machiavellianism related to theory of mind?". Journal of Evolutionary Psychology. 8 (3): 261–274. doi:10.1556/jep.8.2010.3.7. ISSN 1789-2082.
  126. Sutton, J. (2001). Bullies: thugs or thinkers? Psychologist 14, 530–534.
  127. Davies, M., and Stone, T. (2003). “Synthesis: psychological understanding and social skills,” in Individual Differences in Theory of Mind, eds B. Repacholi and V. Slaughter (New York, NY: Psychology Press), 305–353
  128. Esperger, Z., and Bereczkei, T. (2012). Machiavellianism and spontaneous mentalization: one step ahead of others. Eur. J. Pers. 26, 580–587. doi:10.1002/per.859
  129. Czibor, Andrea; Bereczkei, Tamas (2012). "Machiavellian people's success results from monitoring their partners". Personality and Individual Differences. 53 (3): 202–206. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2012.03.005.
  130. "APA PsycNet".
  131. Jonason, P. K., & Schmitt, D. P. (2012). What Have You Done for Me Lately? Friendship-Selection in the Shadow of the Dark Triad Traits. Evolutionary Psychology, 10(3). https://doi.org/10.1177/147470491201000303
  132. Brewer, Gayle; Parkinson, Madison; Pickles, Alice; Anson, Joshua; Mulinder, Georgia (2023). "Dark Triad traits and relationship dissolution". Personality and Individual Differences. 204: 112045. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2022.112045. S2CID 254775278.
  133. Wilson, David Sloan, David C. Near, and Ralph R. Miller. "Individual differences in Machiavellianism as a mix of cooperative and exploitative strategies." Evolution and Human Behavior 19.3 (1998): 203-212.
  134. Collisson, Brian, Jennifer L. Howell, and Trista Harig. "Foodie calls: When women date men for a free meal (rather than a relationship)." Social psychological and personality science 11.3 (2020): 425-432
  135. Ali F., Chamorro-Premuzic T. (2010). The dark side of love and life satisfaction: Associations with intimate relationships, psychopathy and Machiavellianism. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 228–233.
  136. Studies in Machiavellianism, pg. 3, 93, 210
  137. Brewer, G.; Abell, L. (2017). "Machiavellianism, Relationship Satisfaction, and Romantic Relationship Quality". Europe's Journal of Psychology. 13 (3): 491–502. doi:10.5964/ejop.v13i3.1217. PMC 5590532. PMID 28904597.
  138. Carter, Gregory Louis, Anne C. Campbell, and Steven Muncer. "The dark triad personality: Attractiveness to women." Personality and Individual Differences 56 (2014): 57-61.
  139. Rauthmann, John F.; Kolar, Gerald P. (2013). "The perceived attractiveness and traits of the Dark Triad: Narcissists are perceived as hot, Machiavellians and psychopaths not". Personality and Individual Differences. 54 (5): 582–586. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2012.11.005.
  140. Jauk, Emanuel; Neubauer, Aljoscha C.; Mairunteregger, Thomas; Pemp, Stephanie; Sieber, Katharina P.; Rauthmann, John F. (2016). "How Alluring Are Dark Personalities? The Dark Triad and Attractiveness in Speed Dating". European Journal of Personality. 30 (2): 125–138. doi:10.1002/per.2040. S2CID 14542188.
  141. Borráz-León, Javier I.; Rantala, Markus J. (2021). "Does the Dark Triad predict self-perceived attractiveness, mate value, and number of sexual partners both in men and women?". Personality and Individual Differences. 168: 110341. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2020.110341. S2CID 224989942.
  142. "APA PsycNet". psycnet.apa.org.
  143. James, Oliver (7 February 2013). Office Politics: How to Thrive in a World of Lying, Backstabbing and Dirty Tricks. Random House. ISBN 9781409005575.
  144. Dugan, Riley; Rouziou, Maria; Hochstein, Bryan (December 2019). ""It is better to be loved than feared: Machiavellianism and the dark side of internal networking"". Marketing Letters. 30 (3–4): 261–274. doi:10.1007/s11002-019-09503-w. ISSN 0923-0645. S2CID 210553791.
  145. Calhoon, R. P. (1 June 1969). "Niccolo Machiavelli and the Twentieth Century Administrator". Academy of Management Journal. 12 (2): 205–212. doi:10.2307/254816. JSTOR 254816.
  146. Kiazad, Kohyar; Restubog, Simon Lloyd D.; Zagenczyk, Thomas J.; Kiewitz, Christian; Tang, Robert L. (August 1, 2010). "In pursuit of power: The role of authoritarian leadership in the relationship between supervisors' Machiavellianism and subordinates' perceptions of abusive supervisory behavior". Journal of Research in Personality. 44 (4): 512–519. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2010.06.004 via ScienceDirect.
  147. Ellen, B. Parker; Alexander, Katherine C.; Mackey, Jeremy D.; McAllister, Charn P.; Carson, Jack E. (December 2021). "Portrait of a workplace deviant: A clearer picture of the Big Five and Dark Triad as predictors of workplace deviance". Journal of Applied Psychology. 106 (12): 1950–1961. doi:10.1037/apl0000880. ISSN 1939-1854. PMID 33600196. S2CID 231964378.
  148. No, a. i. (2018). “Cheating under pressure: A self-protection model of workplace cheating behavior”: Correction to mitchel et al. (2017). Journal of Applied Psychology, 103(1), 36. doi:10.1037/apl0000275
  149. Shu, L. L., Gino, F., & Bazerman, M. H. (2011). "Dishonest deed, clear conscience: When cheating leads to moral disengagement and motivated forgetting". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 330–349 doi:10.1177/0146167211398138
  150. Djurdjevic, Emilija; Rosen, Christopher C.; Conroy, Samantha A.; Rawski, Shannon L.; Sosna, Katherine U (June 2019). "The influence of political climate on job pursuit intentions and the moderating effect of Machiavellianism". International Journal of Selection and Assessment. 27 (2): 180–192. doi:10.1111/ijsa.12242. ISSN 0965-075X. S2CID 151216697.
  151. Brownell, Katrina M.; McMullen, Jeffery S.; O'Boyle, Ernest H. (2021-05-01). "Fatal attraction: A systematic review and research agenda of the dark triad in entrepreneurship". Journal of Business Venturing. 36 (3): 106106. doi:10.1016/j.jbusvent.2021.106106. ISSN 0883-9026. S2CID 233542634.
  152. Hmieleski, Keith M.; Lerner, Daniel A. (October 10, 2016). "The Dark Triad and Nascent Entrepreneurship: An Examination of Unproductive versus Productive Entrepreneurial Motives". Journal of Small Business Management. 54: 7–32. doi:10.1111/jsbm.12296. S2CID 152230559 via CrossRef.
  153. Gürlek, Mert (2021). "Shedding light on the relationships between Machiavellianism, career ambition, and unethical behavior intention". Ethics & Behavior. 31: 38–59. doi:10.1080/10508422.2020.1764846. S2CID 225719181.
  154. Spurk, D., Keller, A. C., & Hirschi, A. (2016). Do bad guys get ahead or fall behind? Relationships of the dark triad of personality with objective and subjective career success. Social psychological and personality science, 7(2), 113-121.
  155. Satornino, C. B., Allen, A., Shi, H., & Bolander, W. (2023). Understanding the Performance Effects of “Dark” Salesperson Traits: Machiavellianism, Narcissism, and Psychopathy. Journal of Marketing, 87(2), 298–318. https://doi.org/10.1177/00222429221113254
  156. Ricks, J., Fraedrich, J. The Paradox of Machiavellianism: Machiavellianism May Make for Productive Sales but Poor Management Reviews. Journal of Business Ethics 20, 197–205 (1999). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005956311600
  157. Jonason, P. K., Slomski, S., & Partyka, J. (2012). The Dark Triad at work: How toxic employees get their way. Personality and Individual Differences, 52(3), 449–453. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.11.008
  158. Fehr, B.; Samsom, D.; and Paulhus, D. L., (1992). "The Construct of Machiavellianism: Twenty Years Later". In C. D. Spielberger & J. N. Butcher (Eds), Advances in Personality Assessment (Vol 9), pp. 77–116. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  159. Monaghan, Conal; Bizumic, Boris; Sellbom, Martin (2016). "The role of Machiavellian views and tactics in psychopathology". Personality and Individual Differences. 94: 72–81. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2016.01.002.
  160. Monaghan, Conal; Bizumic, Boris; Sellbom, Martin (2018). "Nomological network of two-dimensional Machiavellianism". Personality and Individual Differences. 130: 161–173. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2018.03.047. S2CID 150165701.
  161. Rauthmann, John F., and Theresa Will. "Proposing a multidimensional Machiavellianism conceptualization." Social Behavior and Personality: an international journal 39.3 (2011): 391-403.
  162. Dahling, Jason J.; Whitaker, Brian G.; Levy, Paul E. (2009). "The Development and Validation of a New Machiavellianism Scale". Journal of Management. 35 (2): 219–257. doi:10.1177/0149206308318618. S2CID 54937924.
  163. Wörsdörfer, M. (2014). Inside the ‘Homo Oeconomicus Brain’: Towards a reform of the economics curriculum?. Journal of Business Ethics Education, 11, 1-36.
  164. Wilson, David Sloan, David Near, and Ralph R. Miller. "Machiavellianism: a synthesis of the evolutionary and psychological literatures." Psychological bulletin 119, no. 2 (1996): 285.
  165. Dahling, J. J., Whitaker, B. G., & Levy, P. E. (2009). The development and validation of a new Machiavellianism scale. Journal of management, 35(2), 219-257.

Further reading

  • Christie, R., & Geis, F. L. (2013). Studies in machiavellianism. Academic Press.
  • Jones, Daniel N., and Delroy L. Paulhus. "Machiavellianism." (2009).
  • Fehr, B., & Samsom, D. (2013). The construct of machiavellianism: tvventy years later. Advances in personality assessment, 9, 77.
  • Paulhus, Delroy L., and Kevin M. Williams. "The dark triad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy." Journal of research in personality 36.6 (2002): 556-563.
  • Furnham, Adrian, Steven C. Richards, and Delroy L. Paulhus. "The Dark Triad of personality: A 10-year review." Social and personality psychology compass 7.3 (2013): 199-216.
  • Lyons, M. (2019). The dark triad of personality: Narcissism, machiavellianism, and psychopathy in everyday life. Academic Press.
  • Bereczkei, Tamás. Machiavellianism: The psychology of manipulation. Routledge, 2017.
This article is issued from Wikipedia. The text is licensed under Creative Commons - Attribution - Sharealike. Additional terms may apply for the media files.