Levenson Self-Report Psychopathy Scale
The Levenson Self-Report Psychopathy scale (LSRP) is a 26-item, 4-point Likert scale, self-report inventory to measure primary and secondary psychopathy in non-institutionalised populations. It was developed in 1995 by Michael R. Levenson, Kent A. Kiehl and Cory M. Fitzpatrick. The scale was created for the purpose of conducting a psychological study examining antisocial disposition among a sample of 487 undergraduate students attending psychology classes at the University of California, Davis.[1]
Primary and Secondary Psychopathy
Background
Benjamin Karpman first theorised that psychopathy should be divided into two clinical subtypes in 1941.[2] He believed that psychopathy presented itself in either a symptomatic or idiopathic manner. Symptomatic psychopathy referred to an individual who would exhibit psychopathic traits usually as a result of an underlying psychoneurosis or character neurosis. Idiopathic psychopathy, on the other hand, presented itself without a cause and rarely reacted to treatment. [3] Karpman's theory has helped researchers to identify two subgroups of those who display psychopathic traits. These were subsequently labelled as primary and secondary psychopaths.[4]
Primary Psychopathy
The subtype known as "primary" psychopathy refers to individuals who are completely rational, lack anxiety and have high levels of interpersonal charm. Whilst these behaviours appear incredibly adaptive, primary psychopaths are also prone to dysfunctional and pathological traits such as an inability to learn from past mistakes and a lack of responsibility.[4] Primary psychopaths may present few symptoms of having a major psychological disorder. This may go hand in hand with the more manipulative personality traits characterising primary psychopathic behaviour. In addition, manipulation is also a key component featured in the Dark Triad personality test (Machiavellianism), which is positively correlated with primary psychopathy.[5] It has also been further suggested that primary psychopathy and Machiavellianism reflect similar constructs, with Machiavellian behaviour being positively associated with the main traits of primary psychopathy.[5][6] Further research has shown that psychopathy and Machiavellianism are "barely distinguishable" and features of the two often "overlap".[6] This is further supported by research showing that Machiavellianism and primary psychopathy are situated in much of the same areas of the Interpersonal Circumplex., i.e., "cold-hearted" and "high dominance" sections.[5] Other traits include impaired interpersonal skills, not being able to function adequately in social situations due to the mere nature of the traits such as a lack of empathy.
Secondary Psychopathy
"Secondary" psychopaths are individuals not dissimilar to primary psychopaths in the sense that they still share many of the same characteristics and traits. However, unlike the primary psychopath, the secondary psychopath is more likely to suffer from intense emotional arousal and psychological issues. As well as this, research conducted on adult psychopaths has suggested that secondary psychopaths are more prone to participate in drug abuse, suicide and interpersonal aggression. Research suggests that secondary psychopathic traits may include remorse and being more tense in contrast to individuals labelled as primary psychopaths. Karpman believed that behaviours carried out by secondary psychopaths aren’t grounded in lack of empathy which is seen in primary psychopaths.[7] An integral finding amongst secondary psychopathic traits is the tendency to be less risk averse and more risk tolerant. For example, a study showed that individuals with secondary psychopathic traits were more likely to consistently choose cards with considerably higher net loses opposed to ones that posed less risk and gain.[7] This is consistent with other research, which also suggests a consistent positive association between impulsivity and secondary psychopathy. Whilst controlling for impulsivity, risky decision making was still prevalent amongst secondary psychopathic behaviour.[7][8] Overall, what differentiates secondary psychopaths from primary psychopaths is their destructive behaviour as well an increased reactivity and impulsivity and an inability to control their emotions effectively.[4]
Evaluation
Studies have supported the existence of these two subgroups. For example, after, a questionnaire assessing personality was distributed to 96 male psychopathic male prisoners, the researchers conducting the study concluded the best-fitting model for the differences in personality was two separate groups. One group was labelled "emotionally stable psychopaths" and the other was labelled "aggressive psychopaths". The aggressive psychopaths were more emotionally reactive and lacked control whereas the emotionally stable psychopaths had high levels of achievement and social skills and low levels of stress reaction.[9]
Development and Scoring
The purpose of the scale was to test for these two sub-groups of primary and secondary psychopathy as identified by Karpman as well as their attributions with "prosocial and antisocial behaviours". This was aimed at “non-institutionalised” populations in Levenson’s study.[1][7]
The LSRP is partially based on Robert D. Hare's diagnostic Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R) and consists of 26 statements which participants must decide their attitudes towards using a 4-point Likert scale ("disagree strongly", "disagree somewhat", "agree somewhat" and "agree strongly"). Seven of the items were reversed to reduce response bias.[10]
The PCL-R is one of the key assessments used to signal psychopathy and traits associated with it in individuals, mainly used on institutionalised individuals in the population in contrast to the LSRP. The LSRP aimed to measure similarly what the PCL-R does by using similar “descriptors” for behavioural traits. These include tendency for regular deception, lack of empathy and remorse and Machiavellian behaviour.[1]
Examples of items from the self-report scale include: "I tell other people what they want to hear so that they will do what I want them to do" and "I often admire a really clever scam".[1] These statements aimed to focus on typical moral dilemmas one may face as opposed to accentuating criminal behaviour.[1][10] This was to account for the LSRP’s use on university students, making the scale easier to conceptualise.[11] The wording of these response options is made to lessen the stigma around personality traits by reducing any means of condemnation.[1][7]
A factor analysis was initially conducted on the items in the LSRP and two factors (primary and secondary psychopathy) were derived from the scale.[10] 16 of the statements from the scale determined primary psychopathy and the remaining 10 determined secondary psychopathy. The statements that were attributed to primary psychopathy were more to do with manipulation and a lack of empathy whereas the statements attributed to secondary psychopathy were focused on behavioural issues.[1]: 152–153
Based on the results of the initial study, the following thresholds for the results were established:
- ≤48: Non-psychopathic group
- 49-57: Mixed group
- ≥58: Psychopathic group[10]
Criticism
Construct validity
The LSRP intends to measure the same constructs as the PCL-R. Significant correlations suggest that the LSRP does correlate with the PCL-R for both factors of psychopathy. A study by Chad Brinkley in 2001 tested the concurrent and construct validity of the LSRP with the PCL-R with a sample of prison inmates.[10] Small to moderate correlations were found between the LSRP and PCL-R, as well as associations between primary and secondary psychopathy ("factor 1" and "factor 2" in the PCL-R). Although the total correlations between the two measures were small, this was still nevertheless a significant finding from further statistical testing. This finding was also predominantly consistent across different demographics within the sample. However, these correlations being small to moderate suggest that the LSRP and PCL-R are measuring somewhat different constructs.[10]: 1035
A study by Martin Sellbom in 2010 also studied male inmates and college students to assess construct validity in the LSRP.[12] It was compared with various other measures such as the "Psychopathic Personality Inventory” (PPI), the “Machiavellianism Inventory-IV” and the “Emotional Empathy Scale”. Results signalled the LSRP had strong correlations with some aspects of these measures which tested for narcissistic, impulsive traits and prevalence of substance abuse.[12] There were overall moderate correlations between the LSRP and the PPI but no significant correlations with the Emotional Empathy Scale. The LSRP therefore partially correlates with other predominant measures of psychopathy, but these correlations are not consistent across all self-report measures, with some differences seen between institutionalised and non-institutionalised populations.[10][12]
Further research tested the LSPR against a range of other self-report measures which explored concepts associated with psychopathic behavioural traits. After analysing this, findings showed that there were correlations between the LSRP and other self-report measures assessing similar conducts such as "harm avoidance".[1][10]
Internal validity
The LSRP suffers much of the same problems as other self-reported data. As the participant is completing the form themselves, they are more likely to be subject to biases that can change how they answer questions and thus jeopardise the internal validity of the data. An example of such is the recall bias in which participants must recall what they believe to be relevant information to complete the self-report. However, without the presence of an interviewer who is able to guide the participants thought process, the participant may be using incorrect information that they called upon to answer a question. The potential for this problem is much higher in self-reported data than interviews.[13]
Self-report psychopathy measures are susceptible to social desirability bias, in which individuals may alter their given responses according to what they think is socially acceptable or “desired”, therefore possibly giving unrepresentative psychopathy characteristics for that individual. This could stem from certain stigmas surrounding mental health disorders. This threats the internal validity of the assessment as the nature of the questionnaire may produce inaccurate findings of the population’s behavioural dispositions and thus the scale is not measuring what it intends to measure. For example, in Levenson’s study, responses could lead to wrong grouping of individuals, such as being classed as non-psychopathic when actual responses may lead to being put into the psychopathic group due to “underreporting”. However, an analysis into the effects of this bias on the LSRP and other measures found a negative relationship between psychopathy measures and social desirability, indicating that there was limited social desirability bias in the results.[14] Alternatively, it has been suggested that due to dishonesty being a key feature of psychopathy, using self-report methods to assess people will no doubt lead to mistrust in results.[14]
The cognitive burden of self-reported data is also higher than interviews. The LSRP, being a visual questionnaire, requires literacy skills, a lack of visual impairment and ability to use your hands as well as the ability to follow instructions. Compared to interviews, this increased cognitive burden could result in a lower quality of data.[13]: 281–282
External validity
To create the LSRP, Levenson et al. only used undergraduate students studying psychology at a specific university. This is an example of selection bias and is likely to have negative implications for the external validity of the scale as the wider population does not consist of only undergraduate students.[15] The development of the LSRP also used a WEIRD sample. This refers to a sample from a western, educated, industrialised, rich, democratic society. The problem with this is that due to the restricted sample, the LSRP may not be generalisable to the other 88% of the population who do not live in WEIRD societies, once again decreasing the external validity of the scale.[16]
Internal consistency reliability
Findings on the internal consistency reliability of the LSRP are mixed. A study in 2007 found Cronbach's alpha and mean interitem correlations were sufficient to determine good internal consistency reliability for the total LSRP score as well as the subgroup scores.[17] However, a slightly later study found that the reliability of the items in the LSRP was low and attributed this to a few items in the scale. Once these items were removed, reliability improved.[11]
References
- Levenson, Michael R.; Kiehl, Kent A.; Fitzpatrick, Cory M. (1995). "Assessing Psychopathic Attributes in a Noninstitutionalized Population". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 68 (1): 151–152. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.68.1.151. PMID 7861311.
- Karpman, Ben (1941). "On the need of separating psychopathy into two distinct clinical types: the symptomatic and the idiopathic". Journal of Criminal Psychopathology. 3: 112–137. Retrieved 14 April 2020.
- Arieti, Silvano (October 1963). "Psychopathic personality: Some Views on Its Psychopathology and Psychodynamics". Comprehensive Psychiatry. 4 (5): 301–303. doi:10.1016/S0010-440X(63)80056-5. PMID 14055428.
- Vaughn, Michael G.; Edens, John F.; Howard, Matthew O.; Smith, Shannon Toney (July 2009). "An Investigation of Primary and Secondary Psychopathy in a Statewide Sample of Incarcerated Youth". Youth Violence and Juvenile Injustice. 7 (3): 173. doi:10.1177/1541204009333792. S2CID 143989190.
- McHoskey, John W.; Worzel, William; Szyarto, Christopher (1998). "Machiavellianism and psychopathy". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 74 (1): 192–210. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.192. ISSN 1939-1315. PMID 9457782.
- Miller, Joshua D.; Hyatt, Courtland S.; Maples-Keller, Jessica L.; Carter, Nathan T.; Lynam, Donald R. (2017). "Psychopathy and Machiavellianism: A Distinction Without a Difference?: Psychopathy and Machiavellianism". Journal of Personality. 85 (4): 439–453. doi:10.1111/jopy.12251. PMID 26971566.
- Dean, Andy C.; Altstein, Lily L.; Berman, Mitchell E.; Constans, Joseph I.; Sugar, Catherine A.; McCloskey, Michael S. (2013). "Secondary psychopathy, but not primary psychopathy, is associated with risky decision-making in noninstitutionalized young adults". Personality and Individual Differences. 54 (2): 272–277. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2012.09.009. PMC 3505104. PMID 23185100.
- Miranda Jr, Robert; MacKillop, James; Meyerson, Lori A.; Justus, Alicia; Lovallo, William R. (2009). "Influence of Antisocial and Psychopathic Traits on Decision-Making Biases in Alcoholics". Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 33 (5): 817–825. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2009.00901.x. PMC 2846168. PMID 19298325.
- Hicks, Brian M.; Markon, Kristian E.; Patrick, Christopher J.; Krueger, Rober F.; Newman, Joseph P. (2004). "Identifying Psychopathy Subtypes on the Basis of Personality Structure". Psychological Assessment. 16 (3): 276–281. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.16.3.276. PMID 15456383.
- Brinkley, Chad A.; Schmitt, William A.; Smith, Steven S.; Newman, Joseph P. (28 September 2000). "Construct validation of a self-report psychopathy scale: does Levenson's self-report psychopathy scale measure the same constructs as Hare's psychopathy checklist-revised?". Personality and Individual Differences. 31 (7): 1025. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00178-1. Retrieved 11 April 2020.
- Gummelt, Haley D.; Anestis, Joye C.; Carbonell, Joyce L. (2012). "Examining the Levenson Self Report Psychopathy Scale using a Graded Response Model". Personality and Individual Differences. 53 (8): 1002–1006. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2012.07.014.
- Sellbom, Martin (2011). "Elaborating on the construct validity of the Levenson Self-Report Psychopathy Scale in incarcerated and non-incarcerated samples". Law and Human Behavior. 35 (6): 440–451. doi:10.1007/s10979-010-9249-x. ISSN 1573-661X. PMID 20953972. S2CID 20110327.
- Bowling, Ann (3 May 2005). "Mode of questionnaire administration can have serious effects on data quality". Journal of Public Health. 27 (3): 287. doi:10.1093/pubmed/fdi031. PMID 15870099. Retrieved 11 April 2020.
- Watts, Ashley L.; Lilienfeld, Scott O.; Edens, John F.; Douglas, Kevin S.; Skeem, Jennifer L.; Verschuere, Bruno; LoPilato, Alexander C. (2016). "Does response distortion statistically affect the relations between self-report psychopathy measures and external criteria?". Psychological Assessment. 28 (3): 294–306. doi:10.1037/pas0000168. ISSN 1939-134X. PMID 26121383.
- Fritz, Kyra N.; Lim, Nicholas K. (2018). "Selection Bias". The SAGE Encyclopedia of Educational Research, Measurement, and Evaluation. Thousand Oaks: SAGE Publications, Inc. pp. 1490–1491. ISBN 9781506326139. Retrieved 12 April 2020.
- Henrich, Joseph; Heine, Steven J.; Norenzayan, Ara (2010). "The weirdest people in the world?". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 33 (2–3): 61–83, discussion 83-135. doi:10.1017/S0140525X0999152X. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-0013-26A1-6. PMID 20550733. S2CID 220918842. Retrieved 12 April 2020.
- Falkenbach, Diana; Poythress, Norman; Falki, Marielle; Manchak, Sarah (December 2007). "Reliability and Validity of Two Self-Report Measures of Psychopathy". Assessment. 14 (4): 344–345. doi:10.1177/1073191107305612. PMID 17986652. S2CID 2293298.