Auditory illusion

Auditory illusions are false perceptions of a real sound or outside stimulus.[1] These false perceptions are the equivalent of an optical illusion: the listener hears either sounds which are not present in the stimulus, or sounds that should not be possible given the circumstance on how they were created.[2]

Humans are fairly susceptible to illusions, despite an innate ability to process complex stimuli. Confirmation bias is believed to be largely responsible for the inaccurate judgments that people make when evaluating information, given that humans typically interpret and recall information that appeals to their own biases.[3] Amongst these misinterpretations, known as illusions, falls the category of auditory illusions. The brain uses multiple senses simultaneously to process information, spatial information is processed with greater detail and accuracy in vision than in hearing.[4][5][6] Auditory illusions highlight areas where the human ear and brain, as organic survival tools, differentiate from perfect audio receptors; this shows that it is possible for a human being to hear something that is not there and be able to react to the sound they supposedly heard. When someone is experiencing an auditory illusion, their brain is falsely interpreting its surroundings and distorting their perception of the world around them.[7]

Causes

Many auditory illusions, particularly of music and of speech, result from hearing sound patterns that are highly probable, even though they are heard incorrectly. This is due to the influence of our knowledge and experience of many sounds we have heard.[8] In order to prevent hearing echo created by perceiving multiple sounds coming from different spaces, the human auditory system relates the sounds as being from one source.[9] However, that does not prevent people from being fooled by auditory illusions. Sounds that are found in words are called embedded sounds, and these sounds are the cause of some auditory illusions. A person's perception of a word can be influenced by the way they see the speaker's mouth move, even if the sound they hear is unchanged.[10] For example, if someone is looking at two people saying "far" and "bar", the word they will hear will be determined by who they look at.[11] If these sounds are played in a loop, the listener will be able to hear different words inside the same sound.[12] People with brain damage can be more susceptible to auditory illusions and they can become more common for that person.[13]

In music

Composers have long been using the spatial components of music to alter the overall sound experienced by the listener.[14] One of the more common methods of sound synthesis is the use of combination tones. Combination tones are illusions that are not physically present as sound waves, but rather, they are created by one's one neuromechanics.[15] According to Purwins,[16] auditory illusions have been used effectively by the following: Beethoven (Leonore Overture), Berg (Wozzeck), Krenek (Spiritus Intelligentiae, Sanctus), Ligeti (Études), Violin Concerto, Double Concerto, for flute, oboe and orchestra), Honegger (Pacific 231), and Stahnke (Partota 12).

In the 2017 film, Dunkirk, Hans Zimmer used a Shepard Tone to create a constant feeling of stress and tension.

Examples

There are a multitude of examples out in the world of auditory illusions. These are examples of some auditory illusions:

  • Binaural beats
  • The constant spectrum melody
  • Deutsch's scale illusion
  • Franssen effect
  • Glissando illusion
  • Illusory continuity of tones
  • Illusory discontinuity
  • Hearing a missing fundamental frequency, given other parts of the harmonic series
  • Various psychoacoustic tricks of lossy audio compression
  • McGurk effect
  • Octave illusion/Deutsch's high–low illusion
  • Auditory pareidolia: hearing indistinct voices in random noise.
  • The Shepard–Risset tone or scale, and the Deutsch tritone paradox
  • Speech-to-song illusion
  • Yanny or Laurel

See also

  • Auditory system
  • Barber pole – auditory illusions compared to visual illusions
  • Diana Deutsch
  • Doppler effect – not an illusion, but real physical phenomenon
  • Holophonics
  • Jean-Claude Risset
  • Musical acoustics
  • Phantom rings
  • Pitch circularity
  • Psychoacoustics
  • Sharawadji effect
  • Tinnitus

References

  1. Scott, Brian L.; Cole, Ronald A. (1972). "Auditory Illusions as Caused by Embedded Sounds". The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 51 (1A): 112. Bibcode:1972ASAJ...51R.112S. doi:10.1121/1.1981302.
  2. "Auditory illusion: How our brains can fill in the gaps to create continuous sound". Science Daily. Science Daily. Retrieved February 20, 2019.
  3. Brundage, Steven (2016). "Fooled By FLUENCY: UNDERSTANDING ILLUSIONS AND MISJUDGMENTS IN MUSIC LEARNING". American Music Teacher. 66 (2): 10–13. ISSN 0003-0112. JSTOR 26385737.
  4. Guttman, Sharon E.; Gilroy, Lee A.; Blake, Randolph (2005). "Hearing What the Eyes See: Auditory Encoding of Visual Temporal Sequences". Psychological Science. 16 (3): 228–235. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00808.x. ISSN 0956-7976. JSTOR 40064206. PMC 1431611. PMID 15733204.
  5. O'Callaghan, Casey (2011). "Lessons from beyond vision (sounds and audition)". Philosophical Studies. 153 (1): 143–160. doi:10.1007/s11098-010-9652-7. ISSN 0031-8116. JSTOR 41487621. S2CID 7486290.
  6. Massaro, Dominic W., ed. (2007). "What Are Musical Paradox and Illusion?" (PDF). American Journal of Psychology. University of California, Santa Cruz. 120 (1): 124, 132. Retrieved 15 November 2013.
  7. KAYSER, CHRISTOPH (2007). "Listening with your Eyes". Scientific American Mind. 18 (2): 24–29. doi:10.1038/scientificamericanmind0407-24. ISSN 1555-2284. JSTOR 24939602.
  8. Deutsch, D. (2019). Musical Illusions and Phantom Words: How Music and Speech Unlock Mysteries of the Brain. Oxford University Press. ISBN 9780190206833. LCCN 2018051786.
  9. Deutsch, Diana (2010), "Auditory Illusions", Encyclopedia of Perception, 2455 Teller Road, Thousand Oaks California 91320 United States: SAGE Publications, Inc., doi:10.4135/9781412972000.n56, ISBN 978-1-4129-4081-8, retrieved 2020-11-08{{citation}}: CS1 maint: location (link)
  10. "Auditory Illusions: How your ears can be fooled". hear.com. Retrieved 2019-04-19.
  11. "Do You Hear What I Hear? Amazing Auditory Illusions Explained". IFLScience. Retrieved 2019-04-21.
  12. Scott, Brian L.; Cole, Ronald A. (1972-01-01). "Auditory Illusions as Caused by Embedded Sounds". The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 51 (1A): 112. Bibcode:1972ASAJ...51R.112S. doi:10.1121/1.1981302. ISSN 0001-4966.
  13. Fukutake, Toshio; Hattori, Takamichi (1998-11-01). "Auditory illusions caused by a small lesion in the right medial geniculate body". Neurology. 51 (5): 1469–1471. doi:10.1212/WNL.51.5.1469. ISSN 0028-3878. PMID 9818885. S2CID 8928159.
  14. Begault, Durand R. (1990). "The Composition of Auditory Space: Recent Developments in Headphone Music". Leonardo. 23 (1): 45–52. doi:10.2307/1578465. ISSN 0024-094X. JSTOR 1578465. S2CID 191375886.
  15. Kendall, Gary S.; Haworth, Christopher; Cádiz, Rodrigo F. (2014). "Sound Synthesis with Auditory Distortion Products". Computer Music Journal. 38 (4): 5–23. doi:10.1162/COMJ_a_00265. ISSN 0148-9267. JSTOR 24265446. S2CID 15744586.
  16. Purwins, Hendrik (2005). Profiles of pitch classes circularity of relative pitch and key-experiments, models, computational music analysis, and perspectives (PDF). pp. 110–120.
This article is issued from Offline. The text is licensed under Creative Commons - Attribution - Sharealike. Additional terms may apply for the media files.