Coincidence detection in neurobiology
Coincidence detection is a neuronal process in which a neural circuit encodes information by detecting the occurrence of temporally close but spatially distributed input signals. Coincidence detectors influence neuronal information processing by reducing temporal jitter and spontaneous activity, allowing the creation of variable associations between separate neural events in memory.[1] The study of coincidence detectors has been crucial in neuroscience with regards to understanding the formation of computational maps in the brain.
Principles of coincidence detection
Coincidence detection relies on separate inputs converging on a common target. For example (Fig. 1), in a basic neural circuit with two input neurons—A and B—that have excitatory synaptic terminals converging on a single output neuron (C), if each input neuron's EPSP is sub-threshold for an action potential at C, then C cannot fire unless the two inputs from A and B are temporally close. The synchronous arrival of these two inputs may push the membrane potential of a target neuron over the threshold required to create an action potential. Conversely, if the two inputs temporally arrive too far apart, the depolarization of the first input may have time to drop significantly, preventing the membrane potential of the target neuron from reaching the action potential threshold. Hence, the function of coincidence detection is to reduce the jitter caused by spontaneous neuronal activity, and while random sub-threshold stimulations from cells may not often fire coincidentally, coincident synaptic inputs derived from a unitary external stimulus ensure that a target neuron will fire as a result of the stimulus.
Distal coincidence detection
The above description applies well to feedforward inputs to neurons, which provide inputs from either sensory nerves or lower-level regions in the brain. About 90% of interneural connections are, however, not feedforward but predictive (or modulatory, or attentional) in nature. These connections receive inputs mainly from nearby cells in the same layer as the receiving cell, and also from distant connections which are fed through Layer 1. The dendrites which receive these inputs are quite distant from the cell body, and therefore they exhibit different electrical and signal-processing behaviour compared with the proximal (or feedforward) dendrites described above.
In a short section (perhaps 40 µm long) of distal dendrite, the reaction to activations coming in on synapses to the dendritic spines acts to raise the overall local potential with each incoming signal. This rising potential acts against a background of decay in the potential back to the resting level. If sufficient signals are received within a short period of time (i.e. before the overall voltage decays to background), the voltage of the segment will rise above a threshold, giving rise to a non-linear dendritic spike, which travels, effectively undiminished, all the way to the cell body, and which causes it to become partially depolarised.
This is perhaps the most important form of dendritic coincidence detection in the brain. The more easily understood proximal activation acts over much longer time periods, and is thus much less sensitive to the time factor in coincidence detection.
Sound localization
Coincidence detection has been shown to be a major factor in sound localization along the azimuth plane in several organisms. In 1948, Lloyd A. Jeffress proposed that some organisms may have a collection of neurons that receive auditory input from each ear.[2] The neural pathways to these neurons are called delay lines. Jeffress claimed that the neurons that the delay lines link act as coincidence detectors by firing maximally when receiving simultaneous inputs from both ears. When a sound is heard, sound waves may reach the ears at different times. This is referred to as the interaural time difference (ITD). Due to differing lengths and a finite conduction speed within the axons of the delay lines, different coincidence detector neurons will fire when sound comes from different positions along the azimuth. Jeffress' model proposes that two signals even from an asynchronous arrival of sound in the cochlea of each ear will converge synchronously on a coincidence detector in the auditory cortex based on the magnitude of the ITD (Fig. 2). Therefore, the ITD should correspond to an anatomical map that can be found within the brain. Masakazu Konishi's study on barn owls shows that this is true.[3] Sensory information from the hair cells of the ears travels to the ipsilateral nucleus magnocellularis. From here, the signals project ipsilaterally and contralaterally to two nucleus laminari. Each nucleus laminaris contains coincidence detectors that receive auditory input from the left and the right ear. Since the ipsilateral axons enter the nucleus laminaris dorsally while the contralateral axons enter ventrally, sounds from various positions along the azimuth correspond directly to stimulation of different depths of the nucleus laminaris. From this information, a neural map of auditory space was formed. The function of the nucleus laminaris parallels that of the medial superior olive in mammals.[4]
Synaptic plasticity and associativity
In 1949, Donald Hebb postulated that synaptic efficiency will increase through repeated and persistent stimulation of a postsynaptic cell by a presynaptic cell. This is often informally summarized as "cells that fire together, wire together". The theory was validated in part by the discovery of long-term potentiation. Studies of LTP on multiple presynaptic cells stimulating a postsynaptic cell uncovered the property of associativity. A weak neuronal stimulation onto a pyramidal neuron may not induce long-term potentiation. However, this same stimulation paired with a simultaneous strong stimulation from another neuron will strengthen both synapses.[5] This process suggests that two neuronal pathways converging on the same cell may both strengthen if stimulated coincidentally.
Molecular mechanism of long-term potentiation
LTP in the hippocampus requires a prolonged depolarization that can expel the Mg2+ block of postsynaptic NMDA receptors. The removal of the Mg2+ block allows the flow of Ca2+ into the cell. A large elevation of calcium levels activate protein kinases that ultimately increase the number of postsynaptic AMPA receptors. This increases the sensitivity of the postsynaptic cell to glutamate. As a result, both synapses strengthen. The prolonged depolarization needed for the expulsion of Mg2+ from NMDA receptors requires a high frequency stimulation.[6] Associativity becomes a factor because this can be achieved through two simultaneous inputs that may not be strong enough to activate LTP by themselves.
Besides the NMDA-receptor based processes, further cellular mechanisms allow of the association between two different input signals converging on the same neuron, in a defined timeframe. Upon a simultaneous increase in the intracellular concentrations of cAMP and Ca2+, a transcriptional coactivator called TORC1 (CRTC1) becomes activated, that converts the temporal coincidence of the two second messengers into long term changes such as LTP.[7] This cellular mechanism, through calcium-dependent adenylate cyclase activation, might also account for the detection of the repetitive stimulation of a given synapse.
Adenylyl cyclase (also commonly known as adenyl cyclase and adenylate cyclase) has been implicated in memory formation as a coincidence detector.[8][9][10][11]
Molecular mechanism of long-term depression
Long-term depression also works through associative properties although it is not always the reverse process of LTP. LTD in the cerebellum requires a coincident stimulation of parallel fibers and climbing fibers. Glutamate released from the parallel fibers activates AMPA receptors which depolarize the postsynaptic cell. The parallel fibers also activate metabotropic glutamate receptors that release the second messengers IP3 and DAG. The climbing fibers stimulate a large increase in postsynaptic Ca2+ levels when activated. The Ca2+, IP3, and DAG work together in a signal transduction pathway to internalize AMPA receptors and decrease the sensitivity of the postsynaptic cell to glutamate.[6]
See also
References
- Marsálek, P.; Koch, C.; Maunsell, J. (1997). "On the relationship between synaptic input and spike output jitter in individual neurons". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 94 (2): 735–740. Bibcode:1997PNAS...94..735M. doi:10.1073/pnas.94.2.735. PMC 19583. PMID 9012854.
- Jeffress, L. A. (1948). "A place theory of sound localization". Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 41 (1): 35–39. doi:10.1037/h0061495. PMID 18904764.
- Carr, C. E.; Konishi, M. (1988). "Axonal delay lines for time measurement in the owl's brainstem". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 85 (21): 8311–8315. Bibcode:1988PNAS...85.8311C. doi:10.1073/pnas.85.21.8311. PMC 282419. PMID 3186725.
- Zupanc, G.K.H. 2004. Behavioral Neurobiology: An Integrative Approach. Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK. pp. 133-150
- Frey, Uwe; Morris, Richard G. M. (February 1997). "Synaptic tagging and long-term potentiation". Nature. 385 (6616): 533–536. Bibcode:1997Natur.385..533F. doi:10.1038/385533a0. ISSN 0028-0836. PMID 9020359. S2CID 4339789.
- Purves, Dale (2004). Neuroscience (3 ed.). Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates. pp. 575–608. ISBN 9780878937257.
- Kovacs, K. A.; Steullet, P.; Steinmann, M.; Do, K. Q.; Magistretti, P. J.; Halfon, O.; Cardinaux, J. -R. (2007). "TORC1 is a calcium- and cAMP-sensitive coincidence detector involved in hippocampal long-term synaptic plasticity". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 104 (11): 4700–4705. Bibcode:2007PNAS..104.4700K. doi:10.1073/pnas.0607524104. PMC 1838663. PMID 17360587.
- Willoughby, Debbie; Cooper, Dermot M. F. (July 2007). "Organization and Ca2+ regulation of adenylyl cyclases in cAMP microdomains". Physiological Reviews. 87 (3): 965–1010. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.336.3746. doi:10.1152/physrev.00049.2006. ISSN 0031-9333. PMID 17615394.
- Mons, N.; Guillou, J.-L.; Jaffard, R. (1999-04-01). "The role of Ca2+/calmodulin-stimulable adenylyl cyclases as molecular coincidence detectors in memory formation". Cellular and Molecular Life Sciences. 55 (4): 525–533. doi:10.1007/s000180050311. ISSN 1420-682X. PMID 10357223. S2CID 10849274.
- Hanoune, J.; Defer, N. (2001). "Regulation and role of adenylyl cyclase isoforms". Annual Review of Pharmacology and Toxicology. 41: 145–174. doi:10.1146/annurev.pharmtox.41.1.145. ISSN 0362-1642. PMID 11264454.
- Neve, Kim A.; Seamans, Jeremy K.; Trantham-Davidson, Heather (August 2004). "Dopamine receptor signaling". Journal of Receptor and Signal Transduction Research. 24 (3): 165–205. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.465.5011. doi:10.1081/RRS-200029981. ISSN 1079-9893. PMID 15521361. S2CID 12407397.
Further reading
- Bender, V. A.; Bender, K. J.; Brasier, D. J.; Feldman, D. E. (2006). "Two Coincidence Detectors for Spike Timing-Dependent Plasticity in Somatosensory Cortex". Journal of Neuroscience. 26 (16): 4166–4177. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0176-06.2006. PMC 3071735. PMID 16624937.
- Caillard, O.; Ben-Ari, Y.; Gaiarsa, J. L. (1999). "Long-term potentiation of GABAergic synaptic transmission in neonatal rat hippocampus". The Journal of Physiology. 518 (Pt 1): 109–119. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7793.1999.0109r.x. PMC 2269393. PMID 10373693.
- Joris, P. X.; Smith, P. H.; Yin, T. C. (1998). "Coincidence detection in the auditory system: 50 years after Jeffress". Neuron. 21 (6): 1235–1238. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80643-1. PMID 9883717.
- https://web.archive.org/web/20040519194818/http://bbsonline.cup.cam.ac.uk/Preprints/OldArchive/bbs.neur4.crepel.html
External links
- Auditory Localization by ITD Analysis: The Jeffress Model - Online interactive tutorial (Flash)