Substitutes for Leadership Theory

Substitutes for leadership theory is a leadership theory first developed by Steven Kerr and John M. Jermier and published in Organisational Behaviour and Human Performance in December 1978. [1]

The theory states that different situational factors can enhance, neutralise, or substitute for leader behaviours [2] (Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001). It has received criticism for shortcomings due to perceived methodological issues.[3][4] Empirical research has produced mixed results as to its ability to predict subordinate outcomes.

Origins

Over the years, researchers have developed many leadership theories. Prior to the 1970s, trait leadership theory and path-goal theory were the two heavily researched theories. (Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001). Proponents of trait leadership theory held that the ability to lead is a characteristic some people innately have and others do not. The effort was put forth to uncover which characteristics and abilities leaders had that separated them from non-leaders. Leadership style was another angle researchers took. Proponents of this approach did not believe the ability to lead was innate; rather, it was a set of behaviours anyone could learn (Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001).

In the mid-1970s, a great deal of research was dedicated to the contingency model and path-goal theory. [2] The contingency model stated that various leadership styles would be more or less effective depending on the situation. [2] (Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001; Fiedler, 1965). The path-goal theory proposed that subordinates would be satisfied with their leader if they perceived that their leader's behaviour would bring them future satisfaction. Subordinates would be motivated by their leader if they perceived that completing work tasks would bring them satisfaction and if the leader provided proper coaching, support, rewards, and guidance. [3] Frustrations with not being able to find significant results with anyone's theory resulted in the development of reexaminations and new approaches, including questioning which situations necessitated a leader figure and which did not (Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001).

Steven Kerr and Anne Harlan were two of the researchers who publicly expressed their frustrations with current leadership theories. [4] In the 1970s, Kerr was at Ohio State University, actively involved in leadership research. Kerr was studying existing proposed subordinate constructs such as organisational independence [1] and proposed leadership constructs such as consideration and initiating structure (put forth by the Ohio State Leadership Studies). [5]

Subordinates who are organisational independents do not feel tied to one company and are internally motivated instead of motivated by the characteristics of the organisation, such as leader behavior. [6] [2] Leaders are high on initiating structure to clarify their own roles and their subordinates’ roles in obtaining a goal. [5] Kerr and colleagues noticed many studies had found significant moderators that played a part in the relationship between initiating structure, leader behaviour, and subordinate outcomes. For example, when a task was ambiguous, the relationship between the leader initiating structure and subordinate satisfaction was stronger than if the task was clear. These findings made it apparent that there were variables that affected the relationship between leader behaviour and subordinate outcomes, making the relationship stronger or weaker. [5]

In 1973, Kerr was the first to coin substitutes for leadership as elements in the work setting that lessened leader effectiveness on subordinate outcomes. [6] Further publications [5] led to Kerr and Jermier's 1978 paper, which unveiled substitutes for leadership theory. This paper presented two types of elements in the job environment: substitutes and neutralizers. These elements were proposed to serve as moderators in the relationship between leader behaviour and subordinate outcomes. [7]

The theory originally classified substitutes as characteristics of the subordinate, characteristics of the task, and characteristics of the organization. Howell, Dorfman, and Kerr proposed an alteration to the theory in terms of subordinate classification. They argued that moderators should be grouped based on their effect on the criteria. The original theory had already proposed moderators that act as substitutes and moderators that act as neutralizers. Howell and colleagues added enhancers to these. [8]

[9]

Substitutes for leadership theory were a heavily researched area until the late 1980s, when transformational leadership became the focus of the majority of leadership research.  

Definitions

Substitutes for leadership theory

Substitutes for leadership theory states that different situational factors can enhance, neutralize, or substitute for leader behaviors[10] (Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001).

Substitutes

Substitutes are variables that make leadership unnecessary for subordinates[11] and reduce the extent to which subordinates rely on their leader[12]

Examples of substitutes

  • Characteristics of the subordinate
    • Subordinate ability[13]
    • Subordinate's professional orientation[13]
  • Characteristics of the task
    • Unambiguous and routine task (when all subordinates are performing menial labor, there is little role leadership can play; Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001)
    • Task that provides its own feedback as to how well the task is being done[13]
    • Task that is intrinsically satisfying[13]
  • Characteristics of the organization
    • Cohesive work groups (a tight-knit group of employees has less need for a leader; Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001)
    • Organizational formulation (clear job goals that are written down, performance appraisals that are written down;[14] Kerr and Jermier, 1987)
    • Self-managed work teams (employees rely on each other, not their leader)[15]

Enhancers

Enhancers are variables that serve to strengthen leaders influence on subordinate outcomes[16][11] (Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001

Examples of enhancers

    • Characteristics of the subordinate
      • Subordinates having experience (those more experienced will be able to translate even the most ambiguous instructions into results [1])
    • Characteristics of the task
      • Task is non-routine [2].
    • Characteristics of the organisation
      • Having group norms that encourage cooperation with leaders (Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001)
      • A leader having the ability to reward subordinates [1]

Neutralizers

  • Neutralizers are variables that serve to weaken or block the leader's influence on subordinate outcomes [1] (Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001).

Examples of neutralizers

      • Characteristics of the subordinate
        • Subordinates are indifferent when it comes to rewards [1].
      • Characteristics of the organisation
        • Whether or not subordinates are rewarded is not the leader's decision [1].
      • Kerr & Jermier never specified an example of a task characteristic that acts as a neutralizer. [17]

Scales used to measure

Kerr and Jermier[13]

    • The original scale to measure the effects of various substitutes was developed by Kerr and Jermier. They assessed leadership substitutes via a questionnaire that contained thirteen subscales with a total of 55 items. The items were on a Likert scale ranging from 1 (almost always untrue or completely untrue) to 5 (almost always true or almost completely true). The subscales included were:
      1. Ability, experience, training, and knowledge
      2. Professional orientation
      3. Indifference towards organisational rewards
      4. Unambiguous, routine, and methodically invariant tasks
      5. Task-provided feedback concerning accomplishment
      6. Intrinsically satisfying tasks
      7. Organisational formalisation
      8. Organisational inflexibility
      9. Advisory and staff functions
      10. Close-knit, cohesive, interdependent work groups
      11. Organisational rewards are not within the leader's control.
      12. Spatial distance between superiors and subordinates
      13. Subordinate need for independence

Kerr and Jermier tested nine of these subscales in a lab setting and found that they were independent and had adequate internal reliability. They claim that the subscales produce easily interpretable data that describes the extent to which substitutes for leadership are present or absent in a given work situation.  They went on to test their subscales in a field setting using police officers and concluded that the subscales met acceptable standards of reliability and can be used to assess the validity of the substitutes for leadership construct in future studies.

Podsakoff, Niehoff, MacKenzie, and Williams

Podsakoff, Niehoff, MacKenzie, and Williams noted that studies testing the substitutes for leadership model had not been fully supportive of the theory and believed that one reason for this may be that the quality of the scale developed by Kerr and Jermier to measure the substitutes constructs may be to blame. In response to this, they designed their own 74-item list of substitutes for leadership. To test their scale, they administered it to 372 business students. Their analyses of the psychometric properties of the revised measure revealed their scale to be superior to Kerr and Jermier's scale, as evidenced by the better dimensionality and reliability of the revised scale. [18]

Consequences

Kerr and Jermier proposed that substitute variables should render leader behaviours unable to predict subordinate outcomes. [19] Researchers that have tested this characteristic of substitutes have found mixed results. [2] [20]

A study involving hospital personnel found that tasks that gave feedback regarding performance were negatively correlated with job satisfaction. Routine, repetitive work tasks were correlated with organisational commitment. Intrinsically satisfying work, organisational formulation, and cohesive workgroups were substitutes that were significantly correlated with both job satisfaction and organisational commitment. However, they found little evidence that substitutes prevented or replaced a leader's effect on subordinate job satisfaction or organisational commitment. The only substitute that served as a replacement was organisational formulation, which replaced leader behaviour's predictive power in explaining subordinate job satisfaction and organisational commitment. [21]


Another study looked at 1,235 employees working for 265 leaders in a variety of job settings. Among their findings were that intrinsically satisfying tasks were positively related to subordinate satisfaction, indifference to rewards was negatively related to organisational commitment, and the organisational formulation was negatively related to subordinate perceptions of role ambiguity. The researchers in this study concluded that substitutes for leadership were useful in predicting subordinate outcomes, but leader behaviours and substitutes should always be studied together because together they explained about a third of the total variance in subordinate outcomes in their sample. [22]

A more recent study conducted by Dionne and colleagues [2] collected data from 940 subordinates. Unsatisfied with the fact that prior studies had tested substitutes, enhancers, and neutralizers as moderators, they tested the effect of substitutes as mediators as well as moderators. Testing a variety of different substitutes and outcomes, the researchers found only very weak evidence that substitutes make a difference and concluded that leader behaviours are the only important variable in predicting employee outcomes.

Podsakoff, MacKenzie, and Bommer conducted a meta-analysis in which they took the results of 22 studies that examined the main effect of substitutes for leadership on the relationship between leader behaviours and subordinate outcomes. They found evidence supporting the theory that taking into account both leader behaviours and the effect of substitutes accounted for more of the variance in subordinate outcomes than taking into account leader behaviours alone. [23]

Applications

Autonomous work groups

[24]

Bass (1990) suggested that autonomous work groups could substitute for formal leadership. In this scenario, employees are divided into groups that are responsible for managing their own day-to-day work (i.e., collective control over the pace, distribution of tasks, organisation of breaks, recruitment, and training; Gulowsen, 1972). A quasi-experiment found that implementing autonomous workgroups of 8 to 12 shop-floor employees in a manufacturing setting positively affected both the intrinsic and extrinsic job satisfaction of employees while obviating some supervisory positions.  

Self-management

Self-management is defined by Thoresen and Mahoney (1974) as occurring when an individual behaves in a way he would not normally behave and there are no external forces dictating that the person maintain that behavior. Self-management requires self-observation (e.g., keeping a log of what one has discussed with others on the phone), specification of goals (e.g., being responsible for setting one's own schedule and priorities), cueing strategies (e.g., putting a checkout board by the exit to remind an employee to let their secretary know where they are going and when they expect to return), rehearsal (e.g., recording one's presentation to clients and making corrections as needed), self-evaluation (e.g., using a chart to keep track of work quality and quantity), and self-reinforcement (accomplished by building intrinsic rewards for the performance of tasks; Bass, 1990; Manz & Sims 1980). These strategies can be understood as substitutes for leadership. Instead of requiring that a supervisor monitor a subordinate's progress on a work task, a subordinate can self-manage by utilising one of the strategies listed above. This makes the supervisor's guidance unnecessary for the subordinate.

Criticisms and methodological problems with research

Common-source bias

Dionne and colleagues argued that the significant effects of substitutes found in prior studies may be a statistical artefact due to common-source bias, or bias occurring when independent and dependent variables are collected from the same person or group of people. In a study sampling 49 organisations, Dionne and colleagues controlled for the effect of common-source bias and found no moderating or mediating effects of substitutes on the relationship between leader behaviour and group effectiveness. [25]

[26]

In a study by Podsakoff and Mackenzie, the predictor variables, as well as the job attitude and role perception variables, were both taken from individual employees, while the performance measures were taken from supervisors. They found that their predictors accounted for a higher proportion of variance in job attitudes and role perceptions than in employee performance. They posit that the reason for this was that their predictors shared a common source with the job attitudes and role perception criterion measures but not with the employee performance criterion measures. They suggested that common-source bias may not be the only reason for this, but that it should be controlled in future research.  

Lack of longitudinal studies

[27]

Keller conducted a longitudinal study in which he pointed out that the vast majority of studies conducted on substitutes for leadership theory are cross-sectional in nature, making it more difficult to discern a causal relationship between substitutes and their effects on employee performance. Cross-sectional research is focused on finding relationships between variables at a specific point in time, whereas longitudinal studies involve taking multiple measures over a longer period of time. Keller's longitudinal study of the effect of substitutes on research and development teams found that two of the seven substitutes (ability and intrinsic satisfaction) affected team performance over time.  

Conceptual weakness

Yukl (1998) pointed out that it is hard to identify specific substitutes and neutralizers for broad behaviour categories and that an improvement on the theory would be to use more specific behaviours in place of "supportive and instrumental leadership haviors." Yukl said that recent studies testing the theory have used specific behaviours (e.g., contingent reward behaviour and role clarification); however, the development of the theory does not reflect these developments.

Contingency model

The contingency model of leadership was developed by Fred Fiedler in the 1960s after Fiedler spent 12 years collecting data from over 800 groups of employees. This model predicts that the most effective method of leadership will vary depending on the situation. A study that supported the model was one Fiedler did with 48 petty officers and 240 recruiters at a Belgian naval training centre (Fiedler, 1965). Fiedler found that a leader's fit with the group and the task was more important in predicting outcomes than the leader's characteristics. For example, controlling leaders work best with heterogeneous groups with low positional power. [28] (Fiedler, 1965). There are many different theories within the contingency paradigm that differ on what situational factors change leadership effectiveness. The most influential theory within this model is the path-goal theory (Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001).

Path-goal theory

Proposed by Robert House in 1971, path-goal theory predicts that subordinates will be satisfied with their leader if they feel their leader's behavior will lead them to satisfaction. Also, subordinates will be motivated when they feel that their satisfaction depends on their performance and their leader acts in a way to help them reach goals (Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001; House, 1971). Path-goal theory predicts that when goals and the paths to those goals are clear, subordinates may not need leader guidance (Kerr & Jermier, 1978).

See also

References

  1. Kerr, Steven; Jermier, John M. (1978). "Substitutes for leadership: Their meaning and measurement". Organizational Behavior and Human Performance. 22 (3): 375–403. doi:10.1016/0030-5073(78)90023-5.
  2. Schriesheim, Chester A. (1997). "Substitutes-for-leadership theory: Development and basic concepts". The Leadership Quarterly. 8 (2): 103–108. doi:10.1016/S1048-9843(97)90009-6.
  3. House, Robert J. (1996). "Path-goal theory of leadership: Lessons, legacy, and a reformulated theory". The Leadership Quarterly. 7 (3): 323–352. doi:10.1016/S1048-9843(96)90024-7.
  4. Kerr, Steven; Harlan, Anne (1973). "Predicting the effects of leadership training and experience from the contingency model: Some remaining problems". Journal of Applied Psychology. 57 (2): 114–117. doi:10.1037/h0037122. ISSN 1939-1854. PMID 4784755.
  5. Kerr, Steven; Schriesheim, Chester A.; Murphy, Charles J.; Stogdill, Ralph M. (1974). "Toward a contingency theory of leadership based upon the consideration and initiating structure literature". Organizational Behavior and Human Performance. 12 (1): 62–82. doi:10.1016/0030-5073(74)90037-3.
  6. Kerr, Steven (1973). "Ability-and willingness-to-leave as moderators of relationships between task and leader variables and satisfaction". Journal of Business Research. 1 (2): 115–128. doi:10.1016/S0148-2963(73)80002-5.
  7. Kerr, Steven; Jermier, John M. (1978). "Substitutes for leadership: Their meaning and measurement". Organizational Behavior and Human Performance. 22 (3): 375–403. doi:10.1016/0030-5073(78)90023-5.
  8. Howell, Jon P.; Dorfman, Peter W.; Kerr, Steven (1986). "Moderator Variables in Leadership Research". The Academy of Management Review. 11 (1): 88. doi:10.2307/258333. JSTOR 258333.
  9. Dionne, Shelley D.; Yammarino, Francis J.; Howell, Jon P.; Villa, Jennifer (January 2005). "Substitutes for leadership, or not". The Leadership Quarterly. 16 (1): 169–193. doi:10.1016/j.leaqua.2004.09.012.
  10. Avolio, Bruce J.; Walumbwa, Fred O.; Weber, Todd J. (2009-01-01). "Leadership: Current Theories, Research, and Future Directions". Annual Review of Psychology. 60 (1): 421–449. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163621. ISSN 0066-4308. PMID 18651820.
  11. Schriesheim, Chester A. (1997). "Substitutes-for-leadership theory: Development and basic concepts". The Leadership Quarterly. 8 (2): 103–108. doi:10.1016/S1048-9843(97)90009-6.
  12. Kerr, Steven; Schriesheim, Chester A.; Murphy, Charles J.; Stogdill, Ralph M. (1974). "Toward a contingency theory of leadership based upon the consideration and initiating structure literature". Organizational Behavior and Human Performance. 12 (1): 62–82. doi:10.1016/0030-5073(74)90037-3.
  13. Kerr, Steven; Jermier, John M. (1978). "Substitutes for leadership: Their meaning and measurement". Organizational Behavior and Human Performance. 22 (3): 375–403. doi:10.1016/0030-5073(78)90023-5.
  14. Howell, J. P.; Dorfman, P. W. (1981-12-01). "Substitutes for Leadership: Test of a Construct". Academy of Management Journal. 24 (4): 714–728. doi:10.2307/256171. ISSN 0001-4273. JSTOR 256171. PMID 10253689.
  15. Villa, Jennifer R; Howell, Jon P; Dorfman, Peter W; Daniel, David L (2003). "Problems with detecting moderators in leadership research using moderated multiple regression". The Leadership Quarterly. 14 (1): 3–23. doi:10.1016/S1048-9843(02)00184-4.
  16. Howell, Jon P.; Dorfman, Peter W.; Kerr, Steven (1986). "Moderator Variables in Leadership Research". The Academy of Management Review. 11 (1): 88. doi:10.2307/258333. JSTOR 258333.
  17. Podsakoff, Philip M.; Niehoff, Brian P.; MacKenzie, Scott B.; Williams, Margaret L. (1993). "Do Substitutes for Leadership Really Substitute for Leadership? An Empirical Examination of Kerr and Jermier′s Situational Leadership Model". Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 54 (1): 1–44. doi:10.1006/obhd.1993.1001.
  18. Kerr, Steven; Jermier, John M. (1978). "Substitutes for leadership: Their meaning and measurement". Organizational Behavior and Human Performance. 22 (3): 375–403. doi:10.1016/0030-5073(78)90023-5.
  19. Podsakoff, Philip M.; MacKenzie, Scott B. (1995). "An examination of substitutes for leadership within a levels-of-analysis framework". The Leadership Quarterly. 6 (3): 289–328. doi:10.1016/1048-9843(95)90011-X.
  20. Podsakoff, Philip M.; MacKenzie, Scott B. (1995). "An examination of substitutes for leadership within a levels-of-analysis framework". The Leadership Quarterly. 6 (3): 289–328. doi:10.1016/1048-9843(95)90011-X.
  21. Podsakoff, Philip M.; MacKenzie, Scott B.; Bommer, William H. (1996). "Meta-analysis of the relationships between Kerr and Jermier's substitutes for leadership and employee job attitudes, role perceptions, and performance". Journal of Applied Psychology. 81 (4): 380–399. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.81.4.380. ISSN 1939-1854. PMID 8751455.
  22. Wall, T. D.; Kemp, N. J.; Jackson, P. R.; Clegg, C. W. (1986-06-01). "Outcomes of Autonomous Workgroups: A Long-Term Field Experiment". Academy of Management Journal. 29 (2): 280–304. doi:10.2307/256189. ISSN 0001-4273. JSTOR 256189.
  23. Dionne, Shelley D.; Yammarino, Francis J.; Atwater, Leanne E.; James, Lawrence R. (2002). "Neutralizing substitutes for leadership theory: Leadership effects and common-source bias". Journal of Applied Psychology. 87 (3): 454–464. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.87.3.454. ISSN 1939-1854. PMID 12090603.
  24. Podsakoff, Philip M.; MacKenzie, Scott B. (1995). "An examination of substitutes for leadership within a levels-of-analysis framework". The Leadership Quarterly. 6 (3): 289–328. doi:10.1016/1048-9843(95)90011-X.
  25. Keller, Robert T. (2006). "Transformational leadership, initiating structure, and substitutes for leadership: A longitudinal study of research and development project team performance". Journal of Applied Psychology. 91 (1): 202–210. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.91.1.202. ISSN 1939-1854. PMID 16435950.
  26. Hunt, J.G. (1967). "Fiedler's leadership contingency model: An empirical test in three organizations". Organizational Behavior and Human Performance. 2 (3): 290–308. doi:10.1016/0030-5073(67)90023-2.
  • Bass, B. M. (1990). Bass & Stogdill's handbook of leadership: Theory, research, and managerial applications (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Free Press.
  • Den Hartog, D. N., & Koopman, P. L. (2002). Leadership in organizations. In N. Anderson, D. S. Ones, H. K. Sinangil, & C. Viswesvaran (Eds.), Handbook of industrial, work and organizational psychology, Volume 2: Organizational psychology (166-187). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.
  • Fiedler, E. (1965). A contingency model of leadership effectiveness. In H. Proshansky, & B. Seidenberg (Eds.), Basic studies in social psychology (538-551). New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.
  • Gulowsen, J. (1972). A measure of work group autonomy. In L. E. Davis, & J. C. Taylor (Eds.), Design of jobs (374-390). Harmondsworth, UK: Penguin.
  • House, R. J. (1971). A path-goal theory of leader effectiveness. Administrative Science Quarterly, 16, 321-338.
  • Manz, C. C., & Sims, H. P. (1992). The potential for "groupthink" in autonomous work groups. In R. Glaser (Ed.), 'Classic readings in self-managing teamwork: 20 of the most important articles (401-411). King of Prussia, PA: Organization Design and Development.
  • Thoreson, C. E., & Mahoney, M. J. (1974). Behavioral self-control. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
  • Yukl, G. (1998). Leadership in organizations (4th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
This article is issued from Wikipedia. The text is licensed under Creative Commons - Attribution - Sharealike. Additional terms may apply for the media files.