Zeitgeber

A zeitgeber (/ˈtstˌɡbər/) is any external or environmental cue that entrains or synchronizes an organism's biological rhythms, usually naturally occurring and serving to entrain to the Earth's 24-hour light/dark and 12-month cycles.[1][2]

History

The term zeitgeber (German: [ˈtsaɪtˌɡeːbɐ]; lit.'time giver, synchronizer') was first used by Jürgen Aschoff, one of the founders of the field of chronobiology. His work demonstrated the existence of endogenous (internal) biological clocks, which synchronize biological rhythms. In addition, he found that certain exogenous (external) cues, which he called zeitgeber, influence the timing of these internal clocks.

Photic and nonphotic zeitgebers

  • Light (light is a more important zeitgeber than social interactions[3]).
  • Atmospheric conditions
  • Medication
  • Temperature
  • Social interactions
  • Exercise
  • Eating/drinking patterns

Circadian rhythms

Any biological process in the body that repeats itself over a period of approximately 24 hours and maintains this rhythm in the absence of external stimuli is considered a circadian rhythm.[4] It is believed that the brain's suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN), or internal pacemaker, is responsible for regulating the body's biological rhythms, influenced by a combination of internal and external cues.[2] To maintain clock-environment synchrony, zeitgebers induce changes in the concentrations of the molecular components of the clock to levels consistent with the appropriate stage in the 24-hour cycle, a process termed entrainment.[5]

Early research into circadian rhythms suggested that most people preferred a day closer to 25–26 hours when isolated from external stimuli like daylight and timekeeping. However, this research was faulty because it failed to shield the participants from artificial light. Although subjects were shielded from time cues (like clocks) and daylight, the researchers were not aware of the phase-delaying effects of indoor electric lights.[6] The subjects were allowed to turn on light when they were awake and to turn it off when they wanted to sleep. Electric light in the evening delayed their circadian phase; these results became well known.[7] More recent research has shown that adults have a built-in day, which averages about 24 hours; indoor lighting does affect circadian rhythms; and most people attain their best-quality sleep during their chronotype-determined sleep periods. A study by Czeisler et al. at Harvard found the range for normal, healthy adults of all ages to be quite narrow:[8] 24 hours and 11 minutes ± 16 minutes. The "clock" resets itself daily to the 24-hour cycle of the Earth's rotation.[7][9]

Biological rhythms, including cycles related to sleep and wakefulness, mood, and cognitive performance, are synchronized with the body's internal circadian clock.[10] The best way to observe the workings of this clock is to experimentally deprive individuals of external cues like light and social interaction and allow the body to experience a "free-running" environment – that is, one in which there are no zeitgebers to influence the body's rhythms.[10] Under these circumstances, the circadian clock alone modulates the body's biological rhythms.[10] Normally however, external cues like light-dark cycles and social interactions also exert an influence on the body's rhythms. These zeitgebers do so by alerting individuals to changes in the likelihood of possible rewards or threats in the environment. For example, humans are more likely to find food and shelter in the daytime and less likely to detect predators in the nighttime, meaning wakefulness tends to be most fruitful during the day and sleep is the safest activity for the nighttime.[11] Therefore, changes in light and darkness influence the body to rise during the day and become fatigued at night.

There are many different zeitgebers, and their relative influence on an individual at any given time depends on a number of factors, including the presence and operation of other kinds of zeitgebers. For example, Jürgen Aschoff showed that individuals can compensate for the absence of some zeitgebers like natural light by attending to social zeitgebers instead. Specifically, individuals placed in total darkness for four days did not differ on a variety of measures, including body temperature and various psychomotor tasks like time estimation and finger tapping, from individuals placed in an artificial light-dark environment when both groups were given the same strict time schedule.[12] Researchers concluded that social zeitgebers, like meal times and interactions with other people, can entrain biological rhythms in ways similar to those of other common zeitgebers like light.

Psychological effects of changes

Since the internal clock sets itself using zeitgebers, the loss or disruption of an individual's usual zeitgebers can be very disorienting. When an individual experiences significant changes in zeitgebers, such as being irregularly scheduled for the night shift, those changes can have a variety of negative effects. One example of this phenomenon is jetlag, in which traveling to another time zone causes desynchronization in sleep-wake cycles, appetite, and emotions. Such zeitgeber disruptions can also lead to decreased cognitive performance, negative mood, and in some cases, episodes of mental illness.

Cognitive performance

Researchers have shown that the 24-hour circadian clock also influences cognitive performance in a wide variety of paradigms, including serial search, verbal reasoning, working memory tasks, suppressing wrong answers, and manual dexterity.[10][13] Performance on these tasks varies over the course of a day, with each type of task having a unique daily rhythm. For example, the best time to perform a working memory task tends to be midday, while immediate memory is best in the morning, and simple processing is ideally performed in the evening.[13] In addition, individual differences among participants can have an effect on daily rhythms in performance. Studies have found that children perform mental math exercises most successfully in the morning,[14] but young adults' performance peaks in the evening.[15] This variation in the performance of various tasks is attributable to a number of factors, including relative working memory load, change in strategy, hemispheric dominance, ability to suppress wrong answers, age, level of practice, and morningness-eveningness, many of which fluctuate according to time of day.[13] Based on these findings, researchers conclude that factors that disturb circadian rhythms can also affect cognitive performance.

Mood disorders

Disturbances in zeitgebers can exert a negative influence on emotion and mood as well as cognitive functioning.[10] The disturbance of biological rhythms by zeitgebers is theorized to increase risk for some forms of psychopathology. There is strong evidence that individuals with depression experience irregular biological rhythms, including disrupted sleep-wake cycles, temperature, and cortisol rhythms.[16] These findings support the theory first proposed by Ehlers, Frank, and Kupfer in 1988 that says that stressful life events can lead to depressive episodes by disrupting social and biological rhythms, leading to negative symptoms like sleep disturbance that can trigger depression in vulnerable individuals.[1] Recent work has also demonstrated that interventions like light therapy, sleep deprivation, and some pharmacological antidepressants may be effective in treating depression by reordering these rhythms to their natural state.[17] Such interventions influence an individual's mood, body temperature, cortisol levels, and melatonin production, all of which appear to be irregular in depressed individuals.

Social zeitgebers and mood disorders

Some researchers have suggested that the disturbances in biological rhythms present in depressed individuals may actually be the result of previous disruptions in social interactions, which serve as cues for those rhythms.[1] This possibility may help to explain the relationship between stressful life events and the development of mood disorders. For example, newly married or cohabiting couples often need to adjust to each other's sleeping rhythms when beginning to share the same bed for the first time. This adjustment can be difficult and may lead to disruptions in sleep quality and quantity, and possibly increase risk for depression as a result. Researchers have attempted to explore the effect life events that disturb social rhythms might have on depressive symptoms in a number of ways. A number of studies have looked at whether the loss of a spouse, a significant negative life event often associated with increased depressive symptoms, might lead to increased depression via disrupted social rhythms. In addition to grief, bereaved spouses may also be dealing with changes in numerous social zeitgebers. For instance, bereaved spouses may suddenly be faced with changes in meal times, responsibilities for additional chores, social expectations, or simply the reality of living day to day without one's usual conversational partner. Taken together, findings from studies on bereaved spouses indicate that when bereavement is associated with changes in social rhythms, depressive symptoms are likely to increase; however, if bereaved individuals are able to maintain social rhythms after the death of their spouse, increased depression is less likely.[2] These findings suggest that social rhythm stability may not be entirely dependent on life events, but rather has some traitlike elements as well, since some individuals may be more likely to maintain social rhythms than others following the occurrence of a significant life event.

Recent studies have also found a connection between the disruption of social rhythms and the triggering of manic episodes in bipolar disorder.[18] Differentiating between zeitgeber disturbances that lead to depression and those that lead to manic episodes has proven difficult. However, in both unipolar and bipolar depression, the concept of social zeitgebers as potential risk factors has influenced the development of interventions to address this risk. For bipolar disorder, Interpersonal and Social Rhythm Therapy (IPSRT) is meant to regulate and normalize an individual's social rhythms, including meal times, personal relationships, exercise, and social demands. By regulating social rhythms, therapists hope to normalize in turn individuals' biological rhythms. Studies have not found much evidence that IPSRT improves mood, but individuals undergoing IPSRT experience longer periods between bipolar episodes, indicating that normalizing social rhythms may have a preventative effect.[17]

Seasonal affective disorder

Seasonal affective disorder may occur as a result of deficiencies in zeitgebers (such as light) during the winter months that stimulate the reward activation system, resulting in a depressed mood. Some studies have pointed to the hormone melatonin, which is regulated by circadian rhythms, as a possible mechanism.[19] Because circadian clocks synchronize human sleep-wake cycles to coincide with periods of the day during which reward potential is highest  that is, during the daytime[11]  and recent studies have determined that daily rhythms in reward activation in humans are modulated by circadian clocks as well,[11] external influences on those rhythms may influence an individual's mood.

See also

References

  1. Ehlers, Cindy L.; Frank, E.; Kupfer, D. J. (1988). "Social Zeitgebers and Biological Rhythms". Archives of General Psychiatry. 45 (10): 948–52. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1988.01800340076012. PMID 3048226.
  2. Grandin, Louisa D.; Alloy, Lauren B.; Abramson, Lyn Y. (2006). "The social zeitgeber theory, circadian rhythms, and mood disorders: Review and evaluation". Clinical Psychology Review. 26 (6): 679–694. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2006.07.001. PMID 16904251.
  3. Roenneberg, Till; Kumar, C. Jairaj; Merrow, Martha (23 January 2007). "The human circadian clock entrains to sun time" (PDF). Current Biology. 17 (2): R44–R45. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2006.12.011. hdl:11370/2793f316-3fe4-4120-b57b-27ef543865e8. ISSN 0960-9822. PMID 17240323. S2CID 21533227.
  4. Refinetti, R.; Menaker, M. (1992). "The circadian rhythm of body temperature". Physiology & Behavior. 51 (3): 613–37. doi:10.1016/0031-9384(92)90188-8. PMID 1523238. S2CID 27734031.
  5. Toh, Kong Leong (August 2008). "Basic Science Review on Circadian Rhythm Biology and Circadian Sleep Disorders" (Review, Full Text, PDF). Annals Academy Med Singapore. 37 (8): 662–8. PMID 18797559. Retrieved 15 August 2009.
  6. Duffy, Jeanne F.; Kenneth P. Wright, Jr (August 2005). "Entrainment of the Human Circadian System by Light (Review)". J Biol Rhythms. Sage. 20 (4): 326–338. doi:10.1177/0748730405277983. PMID 16077152. S2CID 20140030.
  7. Charles A. Czeisler MD, PhD (1999). "Human Biological Clock Set Back an Hour". Retrieved 23 September 2007. The variation between our subjects, with a 95 percent level of confidence, was no more than plus or minus 16 minutes, a remarkably small range.
  8. Czeisler, CA; Duffy JF; Shanahan TL; Brown EN; Mitchell JF; Rimmer DW; Ronda JM; Silva EJ; Allan JS; Emens JS; Dijk DJ; Kronauer RE (25 June 1999). "Stability, precision, and near-24-hour period of the human circadian pacemaker". Science. 284 (5423): 2177–81. doi:10.1126/science.284.5423.2177. PMID 10381883.
  9. Panda, S.; Hogenesch, J. B.; Kay, S. A. (2002). "Circadian rhythms from flies to human". Nature. 417 (6886): 329–35. Bibcode:2002Natur.417..329P. doi:10.1038/417329a. PMID 12015613. S2CID 4410192.
  10. Kyriacou, Charalambos P.; Hastings, Michael H. (2010). "Circadian clocks: Genes, sleep, and cognition". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 14 (6): 259–267. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2010.03.007. PMID 20418150. S2CID 29030872.
  11. Murray, G.; Nicholas, C. L.; Kleiman, J.; Dwyer, R.; Carrington, M. J.; Allen, N. B.; Trinder, J. (2009). "Nature's clocks and human mood: The circadian system modulates reward motivation". Emotion. 9 (5): 705–16. doi:10.1037/a0017080. PMID 19803592.
  12. Aschoff, J.; Fatranská, M.; Giedke, H.; Doerr, P.; Stamm, D.; Wisser, H. (1971). "Human Circadian Rhythms in Continuous Darkness: Entrainment by Social Cues". Science. 171 (3967): 213–215. Bibcode:1971Sci...171..213A. doi:10.1126/science.171.3967.213. JSTOR 1731431. PMID 5538832. S2CID 40935482.
  13. Carrier, Julie; Monk, Timothy H. (2000). "Circadian Rhythms of Performance: New Trends". Chronobiology International. 17 (6): 719–732. doi:10.1081/CBI-100102108. PMID 11128289. S2CID 17913894.
  14. Rutenfranz J, Helbruegge T. 1957. Über Tageschwankungen der Rechengeschwindigkeit bei 11-jährigen Kindern. Z Kinderheilk. 80:65–82
  15. Blake, M. J. F. (1967). "Time of day effects on performance in a range of tasks". Psychonomic Science. 9 (6): 349–350. doi:10.3758/BF03327842.
  16. Ban, T. A. (1985). "Affective disorders: Biological aspects". Acta Neurologica. London: Oxford University Press. 7 (2): 129–34. PMID 2409751.
  17. Monteleone, P.; Martiadis, V.; Maj, M. (2011). "Circadian rhythms and treatment implications in depression". Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry. 35 (7): 1569–74. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.07.028. PMID 20691746. S2CID 24239162.
  18. Malkoff-Schwartz, S.; Frank, E.; Anderson, B. P.; Hlastala, S. A.; Luther, J. F.; Sherrill, J. T.; Houck, P. R.; Kupfer, D. J. (2000). "Social rhythm disruption and stressful life events in the onset of bipolar and unipolar episodes". Psychological Medicine. 30 (5): 1005–16. doi:10.1017/s0033291799002706. PMID 12027038.
  19. Lam, R.; Levitan, R. (2000). "Pathophysiology of seasonal affective disorder: A review". Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience. 25 (5): 469–480. PMC 1408021. PMID 11109298.
This article is issued from Wikipedia. The text is licensed under Creative Commons - Attribution - Sharealike. Additional terms may apply for the media files.