Trust (social science)
Trust is the willingness of one party (the trustor) to become vulnerable to another party (the trustee) on the presumption that the trustee will act in ways that benefit the trustor.[1][2][3][4] In addition, the trustor does not have control over the actions of the trustee.[1] Scholars distinguish between generalized trust (also known as social trust), which is the extension of trust to a relatively large circle of unfamiliar others, and particularized trust, which is contingent on a specific situation or a specific relationship.[1]
Part of a series on |
Emotions |
---|
|
As the trustor is uncertain about the outcome of the trustee's actions, the trustor can only develop and evaluate expectations. Such expectations are formed with a view to the motivations of the trustee, dependent on their characteristics, the situation, and their interaction.[5] The uncertainty stems from the risk of failure or harm to the trustor if the trustee does not behave as desired.
In the social sciences, the subtleties of trust are a subject of ongoing research. In sociology and psychology, the degree to which one party trusts another is a measure of belief in the honesty, fairness, or benevolence of another party. The term "confidence" is more appropriate for a belief in the competence of the other party. A failure in trust may be forgiven more easily if it is interpreted as a failure of competence rather than a lack of benevolence or honesty.[6] In economics, trust is often conceptualized as reliability in transactions. In all cases, trust is a heuristic decision rule, allowing the human to deal with complexities that would require unrealistic effort in rational reasoning.[7]
Sociology
Sociology claims trust is one of several social constructs; an element of the social reality.[8] Other constructs frequently discussed together with trust include control, confidence, risk, meaning and power. Trust is naturally attributable to relationships between social actors, both individuals and groups (social systems). Sociology is concerned with the position and role of trust in social systems. Interest in trust has grown significantly since the early eighties, from the early works of Luhmann,[9] Barber[10] and Giddens[11] (see[12] for a more detailed overview). This growth of interest in trust has been stimulated by ongoing changes in society, specifically known as late modernity and post-modernity.
Sviatoslav contended that society needs trust because it increasingly finds itself operating at the edge between confidence in what is known from everyday experience and contingency of new possibilities. Without trust, one should always consider all contingent possibilities, leading to paralysis by analysis.[13] In this sense, trust acts as a decisional heuristic, allowing the decision maker to overcome bounded rationality[14] and process what would otherwise be an excessively complex situation. Trust can be seen as a bet on one of many contingent futures, specifically, the one that appears to deliver the greatest benefits. Once the bet is decided (i.e. trust is granted), the trustor suspends his or her disbelief, and the possibility of a negative course of action is not considered at all. Hence trust acts as a reducing agent of social complexity, allowing for cooperation.[15]
Sociology tends to focus on two distinct views: the macro view of social systems, and a micro view of individual social actors (where it borders with social psychology). Similarly, views on trust follow this dichotomy. On one side, the systemic role of trust can be discussed with a certain disregard to the psychological complexity underpinning individual trust. The behavioral approach to trust is usually assumed[16] while actions of social actors are measurable, leading to statistical modelling of trust. This systemic approach can be contrasted[17] with studies on social actors and their decision-making process, in anticipation that understanding of such a process will explain (and allow to model) the emergence of trust.
Sociology acknowledges that the contingency of the future creates dependency between social actors, and specifically that the trustor becomes dependent on the trustee. Trust is seen as one of the possible methods to resolve such a dependency, being an attractive alternative to control.[18] Trust is specifically valuable if the trustee is much more powerful than the trustor, yet the trustor is under social obligation to support the trustee.[19]
Modern information technologies have not only facilitated the transition towards a post-modern society, but have also challenged traditional views on trust. Information systems research has identified that individuals have evolved to trust in technology, demonstrated by two primary constructs. The first consists of human-like constructs including benevolence, honesty and competence, whilst the second employs system-like constructs such as usefulness, reliability and functionality.[20] The discussion surrounding the relationship between information technologies and trust is still in progress as research remains in its infant stages.
Types
Four types of social trust are recognized:[21]
- Generalized trust, or trust in strangers, is an important form of trust in modern society, involving a large amount of social interactions among strangers.[22]
- Out-group trust is the trust an individual has in members of a different group. This could be members of a different ethnic group, or citizens of a different country, for example.
- In-group trust is that which is placed in members of one's own group.
- Trust in neighbors considers the relationships between individuals who share a common residential environment.
Influence of ethnic diversity
Several dozen studies have examined the impact of ethnic diversity on social trust. Research published in the Annual Review of Political Science[21] concluded that there were three key debates on the subject:
- Why does ethnic diversity modestly reduce social trust?
- Can contact reduce the negative association between ethnic diversity and social trust?
- Is ethnic diversity a stand-in for social disadvantage?
The review's meta-analysis of 87 studies showed a consistent, though modest, negative relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust. Ethnic diversity has the strongest negative impact on neighbor trust, in-group trust, and generalized trust. It did not appear to have a significant impact on out-group trust. The limited size of the impact means apocalyptic claims about it are exaggerated.[21]
Psychology
In psychology, trust is believing that the person who is trusted will do what is expected. According to the psychoanalyst Erik Erikson, development of basic trust is the first state of psychosocial development occurring, or failing, during the first two years of life. Success results in feelings of security and optimism, while failure leads towards an orientation of insecurity and mistrust[23] possibly resulting in attachment disorders.[24] A person's dispositional tendency to trust others can be considered a personality trait and as such is one of the strongest predictors of subjective well-being.[25] Trust increases subjective well-being because it enhances the quality of one's interpersonal relationships; happy people are skilled at fostering good relationships.[26]
Trust is integral to the idea of social influence: it is easier to influence or persuade someone who is trusting. The notion of trust is increasingly adopted to predict acceptance of behaviors by others, institutions (e.g. government agencies) and objects such as machines. Yet once again, perceptions of honesty, competence and value similarity[27] (slightly similar to benevolence) are essential.
There are three different forms of trust commonly studied in psychology. Trust is being vulnerable to someone even when they are trustworthy. Trustworthiness are the characteristics or behaviors of one person that inspire positive expectations in another person. Trust propensity is the tendency to make oneself vulnerable to others in general.[28] Research suggests that this general tendency can change over time in response to key life events.[29] Once trust is lost, by obvious violation of one of these three determinants, it is very hard to regain. Thus there is clear asymmetry in the building versus destruction of trust.
Increasingly in recent times, research has been conducted regarding the notion of trust and its social implications:
- In her book,[30] Barbara Misztal attempts to combine all notions of trust together. She describes three basic things that trust does in the lives of people: it makes social life predictable, it creates a sense of community, and it makes it easier for people to work together.
- In the context of sexual trust, Riki Robbins[31] describes four stages. These consist of perfect trust, damaged trust, devastated trust and restored trust.[32]
- In the context of information theory, Ed Gerck defines and contrasts trust with social functions such as power, surveillance, and accountability.[33][34]
- From a social identity perspective, the propensity to trust strangers (see in-group favoritism) arises from the mutual knowledge of a shared group membership,[35][36] stereotypes,[36] or the need to maintain the group's positive distinctiveness.[37]
Despite the centrality of trust to the positive functioning of humans and relationships, very little is known about how and why trust evolves, is maintained, and is destroyed.[38]
One factor that enhances trust among humans is facial resemblance. Through digital manipulation of facial resemblance in a two-person sequential trust game, supporting evidence was found that having similar facial features (facial resemblance) enhanced trust in a subject's respective partner.[39] Though facial resemblance was shown to increase trust, it also had the effect of decreased sexual desire in a particular partner. In a series of tests, digitally manipulated faces were presented to subjects to be evaluated for attractiveness within the context of a long-term or short-term relationship. The results showed that within the context of a short-term relationship, which is dependent on sexual desire, similar facial features caused a decrease in desire. Within the context of a long-term relationship, which is dependent on trust, similar facial features increased the attractiveness of an individual, leading one to believe that facial resemblance and trust have great effects on relationships.[40]
Interpersonal trust literature[38] suggests that trust-diagnostic situations provide a means by which individuals can gauge or alter the level of trust in relationships. Trust-diagnostic situations refer to in trust or "strain-test"[41] situations which test partners' ability to act in the best interests of the other individual or the relationship, simultaneously rejecting that option which is in one's personal self-interest. Trust-diagnostic situations occur throughout the course of everyday life, though can be created by individuals wanting to test the current level of trust in a relationship.[38]
Low trust relationships occur where individuals have little confidence their partner is truly concerned about them or the relationship.[42] Those in low trust relationships tend to make distress-maintaining attributions[43][44] whereby the consequences of partner's negative behavior become of greatest focus, and any impacts of positive actions are minimized. This feeds into the overarching notion that the individual's partner is disinterested in the relationship, and any positive acts are met with skepticism, leading to further negative outcomes.[42]
Distrusting individuals may not always engage in opportunities for trusting relationships. Someone who was subject to an abusive childhood may have been deprived of any evidence that trust is warranted in future interpersonal relationships.[5] An important key to treating sexual victimization of a child is the rebuilding of trust between parent and child. Failure for the adults to validate the sexual abuse contributes to the child's difficulty towards trusting self and others.[45] Moreover, trust can often be affected by the erosion of a marriage.[46] Children of divorce do not exhibit less trust in mothers, partners, spouses, friends, and associates than their peers of intact families. The impact of parental divorce is limited to trust in the father.[47]
Importantly, trust may be placed in non-human agents in addition to human targets. For instance, people may trust in animals,[48] in the scientific process,[49] and in social machines. Trust helps create a social contract that allows humans and domestic animals to live together.[50] Trust in the scientific process is associated with increased trust in innovations such as biotechnology.[49] When it comes to trust in social machines, people are more willing to trust intelligent machines with humanoid morphologies[51] and female cues[52] and when they are focused on tasks (versus socialization)[53] and when they behave in morally good ways.[54][53] More generally, they may be trusted as a function of the machine heuristic—the mental shortcut assuming that machines are less biased, more accurate, and more reliable than are humans[55] such that people may sometimes trust a robot more than a human.[56]
Humans have a natural disposition to trust and to judge trustworthiness of other individuals or groups of humans and things - in developing relationships with potential mentors worthy of trust for example, perhaps as part of interprofessional work in the referral pathway from an emergency department to a hospital ward,[57] or as part of the process and work of building knowledge on whether the new practices, people, and things introduced into our lives are indeed accountable or worthy of investing confidence and trust in (as captured by the empirically grounded construct of 'Relational Integration' within Normalization Process Theory[58][59]). This can be traced in neuroscience terms to the neurobiological structure and activity of a human brain. Some studies indicate that trust can be altered e.g. by the application of oxytocin.[60][61]
Social identity approach
The social identity approach explains trust in strangers as a function of group-based stereotypes or in-group favoring behaviors based on salient group memberships. With regard to ingroup favoritism, people generally think well of strangers but expect better treatment from in-group members in comparison to out-group members. This greater expectation then translates into a higher propensity to trust an in-group rather than out-group member.[35][37][62] It has been pointed out that it is only advantageous to form such expectations of an in-group stranger if they too know the group membership of the recipient.[62]
There is considerable empirical activity related to the social identity approach. Allocator studies have frequently been employed to understand group-based trust in strangers.[35][36][62][63] They may be operationalized as unilateral or bilateral relationships of exchange. General social categories such as university affiliation, course majors, and even ad-hoc groups have been used to distinguish between in-group and out-group members. In unilateral studies of trust, the participant is asked to choose between envelopes containing money that was previously allocated by an in-group or out-group member.[62] They have no prior or future opportunities for interaction, simulating Brewer's notion that group membership was sufficient in bringing about group-based trust and hence cooperation.[64] Participants could expect an amount ranging from nothing to the maximum value an allocator could give out. Bilateral studies of trust have employed an investment game devised by Berg and colleagues where individuals choose to give a portion or none of their money to another.[65] Any amount given would be tripled and the receiver would then decide whether they would return the favor by giving money back to the sender. Trusting behavior on the part of the sender and the eventual trustworthiness of the receiver was exemplified through the giving of money.[37][62]
Empirical research has demonstrated that when group membership is made salient and known to both parties, trust is granted more readily to in-group members than out-group members.[36][62][63] This occurred even when the in-group stereotype was comparatively less positive than an out-group's (e.g. psychology versus nursing majors),[36] in the absence of personal identity cues,[37] and when participants had the option of a sure sum of money (i.e. in essence opting out of the need to trust a stranger).[35] In contrast, when only the recipient was made aware of group membership, trust becomes reliant upon group stereotypes.[36][37] The group with the more positive stereotype was trusted (e.g. one's university affiliation over another)[37] even over that of the in-group (e.g. nursing over psychology majors).[36] Another reason for in-groups favoring behaviors in trust could be attributed to the need to maintain in-group positive distinctiveness, particularly in the presence of social identity threat.[63] Trust in out-group strangers increased when personal cues to identity were revealed.[37]
Philosophy
Whilst many philosophers have written about different forms of trust, most would agree interpersonal trust is the foundation on which these forms can be modeled.[66] For an act to be classed as an expression of trust, it must not betray the expectations of the trustee. In this sense, some philosophers such as Lagerspetz argue that trust is a kind of reliance, though not merely reliance.[67] Gambetta argued it is the inherent belief that others generally have good intentions which is the foundation for our reliance on them.[68] Philosophers such as Annette Baier have contended this view, establishing a difference between trust and reliance by saying that trust can be betrayed, whilst reliance can only be disappointed (Baier 1986, 235).[69] Carolyn McLeod explains Baier's argument by giving the following examples: we can rely on our clock to give the time, but we do not feel betrayed when it breaks, thus, we cannot say that we trusted it; we are not trusting when we are suspicious of the other person, because this is in fact an expression of distrust (McLeod 2006).[66] The violation of trust warrants this sense of betrayal.[70] Thus, trust is different from reliance in the sense that a trustor accepts the risk of being betrayed.
Karen Jones proposed that there is an emotional aspect to trust, an element of optimism[71] that the trustee will do the right thing by the trustor, also described as affective trust.[72] Though, in some circumstances, we trust others even without the optimistic expectation, instead hoping the mere recognition that the person is being trusted in itself will prompt the favorable action. This is known as therapeutic trust[73][74] and gives both the trustee a reason to be trustworthy, and the trustor a reason to believe they are trustworthy. In these situations, the sense of betrayal upon violation of trust is commonly warranted.
The definition of trust as a belief in something or a confident expectation about something[75] eliminates the notion of risk, because it does not include whether the expectation or belief is favorable or unfavorable. For example, to have an expectation of a friend arriving to dinner late because she has habitually arrived late for the last fifteen years, is a confident expectation (whether or not we agree with her annoying late arrivals). The trust is not about what we wish for, but rather it is in the consistency of the data of our habits. As a result, there is no risk or sense of betrayal because the data now exists as collective knowledge. Faulkner[72] contrasts such predictive trust with aforementioned affective trust, proposing predictive trust may only warrant disappointment as a consequence of an inaccurate prediction, not betrayal.
Economics
Trust in economics is treated as an explanation for a difference between actual human behavior and the one that can be explained by the individual desire to maximize one's utility. In economic terms, trust can provide an explanation of a difference between Nash equilibrium and the observed equilibrium. Such an approach can be applied to individuals as well as societies.
Trust is important to economists for many reasons. Taking the "Market for Lemons" transaction popularized by George Akerlof as an example,[77] if a buyer of a car doesn't trust the seller to not sell a lemon, the transaction won't be entered into. The buyer won't enter into the transaction in the absence of trust, even if the product is of great value to the buyer. Trust can act as an economic lubricant, reducing the cost of transactions between parties, enabling new forms of cooperation and generally furthering business activities;[78][79] employment and prosperity. This observation [80] created a significant interest in considering trust as a form of social capital and has led research into closer understanding of the process of creation and distribution of such capital. It has been claimed that a higher level of social trust is positively correlated with economic development. Even though the original concept of 'high trust' and 'low trust' societies may not necessarily hold, it has been widely accepted and demonstrated that social trust benefits the economy [81] and that a low level of trust inhibits economic growth. The absence of trust restricts growth in employment, wages and profits, thus reducing the overall welfare of society.[82]
Theoretical economical modelling[83] demonstrates that the optimum level of trust that a rational economic agent should exhibit in transactions is equal to the trustworthiness of the other party. Such a level of trust leads to an efficient market. Trusting less leads to the loss of economic opportunities, while trusting more leads to unnecessary vulnerabilities and potential exploitation. Economics is also interested in quantifying trust, usually in monetary terms. The level of correlation between an increase in profit margin[84] or a decrease in transactional costs can be used as indicators of the economic value of trust.
Economic 'trust games' are popularly used to empirically quantify trust in relationships under laboratory conditions. There are several games and game-like scenarios related to trust that have been tried, with certain preferences to those that allow the estimation of confidence in monetary terms.[85] Games of trust are designed in a way such that the Nash equilibrium differs from Pareto optimum so that no player alone can maximize his own utility by altering his selfish strategy without cooperation. Cooperating partners can also benefit. The classical version of the game of trust has been described[86] as an abstracted investment game, using the scenario of an investor and a broker. The investor can invest a fraction of his money, and broker can return only part of his gains. If both players follow their economical best interest, the investor should never invest and the broker will never be able to repay anything. Thus the flow of money flow, its volume and character is attributable entirely to the existence of trust. The game can be played as a once-off, or repeatedly, with the same or different sets of players to distinguish between a general propensity to trust and trust within particular relationships. Several other variants of this game exist. Reversing rules lead to the game of distrust, pre-declarations can be used to establish intentions of players,[87] while alterations to the distribution of gains can be used to manipulate perception of both players. The game can be also played by several players on the closed market,[88] with or without information about reputation.
Other interesting games are e.g. binary-choice trust games,[89] the gift-exchange game[90] and various other forms of social games. Specifically games based on the Prisoner's Dilemma[91] are popularly used to link trust with economic utility and demonstrate the rationality behind reciprocity.
The popularization of e-commerce opened the discussion of trust in economy to new challenges while at the same time elevating the importance of trust, and desire to understand customer decision to trust.[92] For example, interpersonal relationships between buyers and sellers have been disintermediated by the technology,[93] and consequentially required improvement.[94] Websites can influence the buyer to trust the seller, regardless of the seller's actual trustworthiness (e.g.[95]). Reputation-based systems improved on trust assessment by allowing to capture the collective perception of trustworthiness, generating significant interest in various models of reputation.[96]
Management and organization science
In management and organization science, trust is studied as a factor that can be managed and influenced by organizational actors. Scholars have paid particular attention to how trust develops across individual and organizational levels of analysis.[97] They suggest a reciprocal process where organizational structures influence individuals’ trust and, at the same time, individuals’ trust manifests in organizational structures. Trust is also one of the conditions of an organizational culture that supports knowledge sharing.[98] An organizational culture that supports knowledge sharing allows employees to feel secure and comfortable to share their knowledge, their work and their expertise.[98][99] Structure often creates trust in a person that encourages them to feel comfortable and excel in the workplace; it makes an otherwise stressful environment manageable. By having a conveniently organized area to work on, concentration will increase as well as effort.
Management and organization science scholars have also paid attention to how trust is influenced by contracts and how trust interacts with formal mechanisms.[100] Parallel to the very large interest in trust, scholars in management and related disciplines have made the case for the importance of distrust as a related but distinct construct.[101][102]
Since the mid-1990s, a substantial body of organizational research has fallen into one of two distinct but nonexclusive paradigms of trust research.[103] The first paradigm distinguishes between two major dimensions of trust. Trust in another can be characterized as cognition-based trust (i.e., based on rational calculation) and affect-based trust (i.e., based on emotional attachment).[104] For example, trust in an auto repair shop could come in the form of an assessment of the capabilities of the shop to do a good job repairing one's car (cognition-based trust) or having a longstanding relationship with the shop's owner (affect-based trust). The second paradigm distinguishes between the trustworthiness factors that give rise to trust (i.e., one's perceived ability, benevolence, and integrity) and trust itself.[3] Together, these paradigms are useful for predicting how different dimensions of trust form in organizations via the demonstration of various trustworthiness attributes.[103]
Systems
In systems, a trusted component has a set of properties which another component can rely on. If A trusts B, a violation in those properties of B might compromise the correct operation of A. Observe that those properties of B trusted by A might not correspond quantitatively or qualitatively to B's actual properties. This occurs when the designer of the overall system does not take the relation into account. In consequence, trust should be placed to the extent of the component's trustworthiness. The trustworthiness of a component is thus, not surprisingly, defined by how well it secures a set of functional and non-functional properties, deriving from its architecture, construction, and environment, and evaluated as appropriate.[105]
See also
- Anticipation
- Attachment theory
- Credulity
- Gullibility
- Intimacy
- Leap of faith
- Misplaced trust
- Personal boundaries
- Position of trust
- Source criticism
- Swift trust theory
- Trust metric
- Trusted system
- Trust in computing
References
- Schilke, Oliver; Reimann, Martin; Cook, Karen S. (2021). "Trust in Social Relations". Annual Review of Sociology. 47 (1): 239–259. doi:10.1146/annurev-soc-082120-082850. ISSN 0360-0572. S2CID 231685149.
- Lynn, Theo (2021). Data privacy and trust in cloud computing : building trust in the cloud through assurance and accountability. Palgrave Studies in Digital Business & Enabling Technologies. Open. Cham: Springer. p. 20. doi:10.1007/978-3-030-54660-1. ISBN 978-3-030-54659-5. OCLC 1202743216. S2CID 242965934.
Trust is generally defined as a willingness to accept vulnerability based on positive expectation of another party.
- Mayer, R.C.; Davis, J.H.; Schoorman, F.D. (1995). "An integrative model of organizational trust". Academy of Management Review. 20 (3): 709–734. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.457.8429. doi:10.5465/amr.1995.9508080335.
- Bamberger, Walter (2010). "Interpersonal Trust – Attempt of a Definition". Scientific report, Technische Universität München. Retrieved 2011-08-16.
- Hardin, Russell (2002-03-21). Trust and Trustworthiness. Russell Sage Foundation. ISBN 978-1-61044-271-8.
- Nooteboom, B. (2017). Trust: Forms, Foundations, Functions, Failures and Figures. Edward Elgar Publishing. ISBN 9781781950883. Retrieved 29 October 2017 – via Google Books.
- Lewicki, Roy; Brinsfield, Chad (2011). "Trust as a heuristic". Framing Matters: Perspectives on Negotiation Research and Practice in Communication. Peter Lang Publishing.
- Searle, J.R. (1995). The Construction of Social Reality. The Free Press
- Luhmann, N. (1979). Trust and Power. John Wiley & Sons.
- Barber, B. (1983) The Logic and Limits of Trust. Rutgerts University Press.
- Giddens, A. (1984). The Constitution of Society: Outline of the Theory of Structuration; Polity Press, Cambridge
- Sztompka, P. (1999). Trust: A Sociological Theory. Cambridge University Press.
- Braynov, Sviatoslav (2002). "Contracting with uncertain level of Trust". Computational Intelligence. 18 (4): 501–514. doi:10.1111/1467-8640.00200. S2CID 33473191.
- Simon, Herbert Alexander (1997). Models of Bounded Rationality: Empirically grounded economic reason. MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-19372-6.
- Bachmann, R. (2001). "Trust, Power and Control in Transorganizational Relations". Organization Studies. 22 (2): 337–365. doi:10.1177/0170840601222007. S2CID 5657206.
- Coleman, J. (1990). Foundations of Social Theory. The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.
- Castelfranchi, C., Falcone, R. (2000). "Trust Is Much More than Subjective Probability: Mental Components and Sources of Trust". Proc. of the 33rd Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences. Volume 6.
- Mollering, G.. "The Trust/Control Duality: An Integrative Perspective on Positive Expectations of Others". In: Int. Sociology, September 2005, Vol. 20(3): 283–305.
- Baier, A. (1986). "Trust and antitrust". Ethics, vol. 96, pp. 231–260. Reprinted in: Moral Prejudices. Cambridge University Press.
- Lankton, Nancy; McKnight, Harrison; Tripp, John (2015). "Technology, Humanness, and Trust: Rethinking Trust in Technology". Journal of the Association for Information Systems. 16 (10): 880–918. doi:10.17705/1jais.00411. ISSN 1536-9323. S2CID 45062514.
- Dinesen, Peter Thisted; Schaeffer, Merlin; Sønderskov, Kim Mannemar (2020). "Ethnic Diversity and Social Trust: A Narrative and Meta-Analytical Review". Annual Review of Political Science. 23: 441–465. doi:10.1146/annurev-polisci-052918-020708.
- Intravia, J.; Stewart, E.; Warren, P.; Wolff, K. (2016). "Neighborhood disorder and generalized trust: A multilevel mediation examination of social mechanisms". Journal of Criminal Justice. 46 (1): 148–158. doi:10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2016.05.003.
- Child Development Institute Parenting Today. "Stages of Social-Emotional Development In Children and Teenagers". Childdevelopmentinfo.com. Archived from the original on October 2, 2011. Retrieved 2013-01-04.
- Fonagy, Peter (2010). Attachment Theory and Psychoanalysis. Other Press Professional, ISBN 1590514602
- DeNeve, Kristina M.; Cooper, Harris (1998). "The Happy Personality: A Meta-Analysis of 137 Personality Traits and Subjective Well-Being" (PDF). Psychological Bulletin. 124 (2): 197–229. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.124.2.197. PMID 9747186.
- DeNeve, Kristina M. (1999). "Happy as an Extraverted Clam? The Role of Personality for Subjective Well-Being". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 8 (5): 141–144. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00033. S2CID 142992658.
- Garcia-Retamero, Rocio; Müller, Stephanie M.; Rousseau, David L. (2012-03-13). "The Impact of Value Similarity and Power on the Perception of Threat". Political Psychology. 33 (2): 179–193. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9221.2012.00869.x. ISSN 0162-895X.
- Colquitt, Jason A.; Scott, Brent A.; LePine, Jeffery A. (2007). "Trust, trustworthiness, and trust propensity: A meta-analytic test of their unique relationships with risk taking and job performance". Journal of Applied Psychology. American Psychological Association. 92 (4): 909–927. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.92.4.909. ISSN 1939-1854. PMID 17638454.
- Van Der Werff, L.; Freeney, Y.; Lance, C. E.; Buckley, F. (2019). "Frontiersin". Frontiers in Psychology. 10: 2490. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02490. PMC 6848461. PMID 31749749.
- Barbara Misztal, Trust in Modern Societies: The Search for the Bases of Social Order, Polity Press, ISBN 0-7456-1634-8
- Riki Robbins, Betrayed!: How You Can Restore Sexual Trust and Rebuild Your Life, Adams Media Corporation, ISBN 1-55850-848-1
- "Four stages of trust". Innerself.com. 2006-09-20. Retrieved 2013-01-04.
- Ed Gerck, Trust Points, Digital Certificates: Applied Internet Security by J. Feghhi, J. Feghhi and P. Williams, Addison-Wesley, ISBN 0-201-30980-7, 1998.
- Ed Gerck (1998-01-23). "Definition of trust". Mcwg.org. Retrieved 2013-01-04.
- Platow, M. J.; Foddy, M.; Yamagishi, T.; Lim, L.; Chow, A. (2012). "Two experimental tests of trust in in-group strangers: The moderating role of common knowledge of group membership". European Journal of Social Psychology. 42: 30–35. doi:10.1002/ejsp.852.
- Foddy, M.; Platow, M.J.; Yamagishi, T. (2009). "Group-based trust in strangers: The role of stereotypes and expectations". Psychological Science. 20 (4): 419–422. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02312.x. PMID 19399956. S2CID 29922902.
- Tanis, M.; Postmes, T. (2005). "A social identity approach to trust: Interpersonal perception, group membership and trusting behaviour" (PDF). European Journal of Social Psychology. 35 (3): 413–424. doi:10.1002/ejsp.256. S2CID 15706516.
- Simpson, Jeffry A. (2016-06-23). "Psychological Foundations of Trust". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 16 (5): 264–268. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00517.x. ISSN 1467-8721. S2CID 45119866.
- Lisa M. DeBruine (7 July 2002). "Facial resemblance enhances trust". Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences. 269 (1498): 1307–1312. doi:10.1098/rspb.2002.2034. PMC 1691034. PMID 12079651.
- DeBruine, Lisa (3 November 2005). "Trustworthy but not lust-worthy: context-specific effects of facial resemblance". Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 272 (1566): 919–22. doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.3003. JSTOR 30047623. PMC 1564091. PMID 16024346.
- Shallcross, Sandra L.; Simpson, Jeffry A. (2012). "Trust and responsiveness in strain-test situations: A dyadic perspective". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 102 (5): 1031–1044. doi:10.1037/a0026829. ISSN 1939-1315. PMID 22250662.
- Rempel, John K.; Ross, Michael; Holmes, John G. (2001). "Trust and communicated attributions in close relationships". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 81 (1): 57–64. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.1.57. ISSN 1939-1315. PMID 11474726.
- Collins, Nancy L. (1996). "Working models of attachment: Implications for explanation, emotion, and behavior". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 71 (4): 810–832. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.71.4.810. ISSN 1939-1315. PMID 8888604.
- Holtzworth-Munroe, Amy; Jacobson, Neil S. (1985). "Causal attributions of married couples: When do they search for causes? What do they conclude when they do?". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 48 (6): 1398–1412. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.48.6.1398. ISSN 1939-1315. PMID 4020604.
- Timmons-Mitchell, Jane; Gardner, Sondra (1991). "Treating sexual victimization: Developing trust-based relating in the mother-daughter dyad". Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training. American Psychological Association. 28 (2): 333–338. doi:10.1037/0033-3204.28.2.333. ISSN 1939-1536.
- Brinig, Margaret F. (2011). "Belonging and Trust: Divorce and Social Capital". SSRN Electronic Journal. doi:10.2139/ssrn.1767431. ISSN 1556-5068.
- King, Valarie (August 2002). "Parental Divorce and Interpersonal Trust in Adult Offspring". Journal of Marriage and Family. 64 (3): 642–656. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3737.2002.00642.x. JSTOR 3599931.
- Billings, Deborah R.; Schaefer, Kristin E.; Chen, Jessie Y.; Kocsis, Vivien; Barrera, Maria; Cook, Jacquelyn; Ferrer, Michelle; Hancock, Peter A. (2012-03-01), Human-Animal Trust as an Analog for Human-Robot Trust: A Review of Current Evidence, University of Central Florida Orlando – via Defense Technical Information Center
- Wang, Zuoming (2017-11-30). "Media, Biotechnology, and Trust: What Drives Citizens to Support Biotechnology". Studies in Media and Communication. 5 (2): 157–165. doi:10.11114/smc.v5i2.2803.
- Oma, Kristin Armstrong (2010-06-01). "Between trust and domination: social contracts between humans and animals". World Archaeology. 42 (2): 175–187. doi:10.1080/00438241003672724. S2CID 219608475.
- Natarajan, Manisha; Gombolay, Matthew (2020-03-09). "Effects of Anthropomorphism and Accountability on Trust in Human Robot Interaction". Proceedings of the 2020 ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction: 33–42. doi:10.1145/3319502.3374839. ISBN 9781450367462. S2CID 212549044.
- Bernotat, Jasmin; Eyssel, Friederike; Sachse, Janik (2019-05-25). "The (Fe)male Robot: How Robot Body Shape Impacts First Impressions and Trust Towards Robots". International Journal of Social Robotics. 13 (3): 477–489. doi:10.1007/s12369-019-00562-7. S2CID 182570618.
- Banks, Jaime; Koban, Kevin; Chauveau, Philippe (2021-04-15). "Forms and Frames: Mind, Morality, and Trust in Robots across Prototypical Interactions". Human-Machine Communication. 2 (1): 81–103. doi:10.30658/hmc.2.4.
- Banks, Jaime (2020-09-10). "Good Robots, Bad Robots: Morally Valenced Behavior Effects on Perceived Mind, Morality, and Trust". International Journal of Social Robotics. 13 (8): 2021–2038. doi:10.1007/s12369-020-00692-3.
- Sundar, S Shyam (January 2020). "Rise of Machine Agency: A Framework for Studying the Psychology of Human–AI Interaction (HAII)". Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication. 25 (1): 74–88. doi:10.1093/jcmc/zmz026.
- Banks, Jaime (2021-01-28). "Of like mind: The (mostly) similar mentalizing of robots and humans". Technology, Mind, and Behavior. 1 (2). doi:10.1037/tmb0000025.
- Sujan, M. A., Huang, H., & Biggerstaff, D. (2019). Trust and psychological safety as facilitators of resilient health care. In Working Across Boundaries (pp. 125-136). CRC Press.
- McEvoy, R., Ballini, L., Maltoni, S., O’Donnell, C. A., Mair, F. S., & MacFarlane, A. (2014). A qualitative systematic review of studies using the normalization process theory to research implementation processes. Implementation Science, 9(1), 1-13.
- May, C. R., Cummings, A., Girling, M., Bracher, M., Mair, F. S., May, C. M., ... & Finch, T. (2018). Using normalization process theory in feasibility studies and process evaluations of complex healthcare interventions: a systematic review. Implementation science, 13(1), 1-27.
- Kosfeld, M.; Heinrichs, M.; Zak, P. J.; Fischbacher, U.; Fehr, E. (2005). "Oxytocin increases trust in humans". Nature. 435 (7042): 673–676. Bibcode:2005Natur.435..673K. doi:10.1038/nature03701. PMID 15931222. S2CID 1234727.
- Zak, Paul; Knack, Stephen (2001). "Trust and Growth". Economic Journal. 111 (470): 295–321. doi:10.1111/1468-0297.00609.
- Biel, Anders; Eek, Daniel; Gärling, Tommy; Gustafsson, Mathias, eds. (2008). New issues and paradigms in research on social dilemmas. New York, N.Y.: Springer. ISBN 978-0-387-72596-3. OCLC 233971331.
- Guth, W.; Levati, M.V.; Ploner, M. (2006). "Social identity and trust – An experimental investigation". The Journal of Socio-Economics. 37 (4): 1293–1308. doi:10.1016/j.socec.2006.12.080. S2CID 142810413.
- Brewer, M.B. (1999). "The psychology of prejudice: Ingroup love or outgroup hate?". Journal of Social Issues. 55 (3): 429–444. doi:10.1111/0022-4537.00126.
- Berg, J., Dickhaut, J., & McCabe, K. (1995). "Trust, reciprocity, and social history". Games and Economic Behaviour, 10, 122–142
- McLeod, Carolyn (2017). Zalta, Edward N. (ed.). The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Metaphysics Research Lab, Stanford University. Retrieved 29 October 2017 – via Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy.
- Lagerspetz, Olli (1998). "Trust: The Tacit Demand". Library of Ethics and Applied Philosophy. 1. doi:10.1007/978-94-015-8986-4. ISBN 978-90-481-4963-6. ISSN 1387-6678.
- Gambetta, Diego (2000). "Can We Trust Trust?". Trust: Making and Breaking Cooperative Relations. Department of Sociology, University of Oxford: 213–237.
- Baier, Annette (1986). "Trust and Antitrust". Ethics. 96 (2): 231–260. doi:10.1086/292745. JSTOR 2381376. S2CID 159454549.
- Hawley, Katherine (2012-10-25). "Trust, Distrust and Commitment". Noûs. 48 (1): 1–20. doi:10.1111/nous.12000. hdl:10023/3430. ISSN 0029-4624.
- Jones, Karen (2005), "Trust as an Affective Attitude", Personal Virtues, London: Palgrave Macmillan UK, pp. 253–279, doi:10.1007/978-0-230-20409-6_11, ISBN 978-1-4039-9455-4, retrieved 2020-11-01
- Faulkner, Paul (2011). Knowledge on Trust. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199589784.001.0001. ISBN 978-0-19-958978-4.
- Horsburgh, H. J. N. (1960-10-01). "The Ethics of Trust". The Philosophical Quarterly. 10 (41): 343–354. doi:10.2307/2216409. ISSN 0031-8094. JSTOR 2216409.
- Pettit, Philip (1995). "The Cunning of Trust". Philosophy & Public Affairs. 24 (3): 202–225. doi:10.1111/j.1088-4963.1995.tb00029.x. ISSN 0048-3915. JSTOR 2961900.
- "trust." Online Etymology Dictionary. Douglas Harper, Historian. 25 May. 2013.
- The Spirit Level: Why More Equal Societies Almost Always Do Better. London, Allen Lane (2009). ISBN 978-1-84614-039-6 UK Paperback edition ISBN 978-0-14-103236-8 (2010)
- Akerlof, George A. (1970). "The Market for "Lemons": Quality Uncertainty and the Market Mechanism". The Quarterly Journal of Economics. 84 (3): 488–500. doi:10.2307/1879431. JSTOR 1879431.
- Morgan, Robert; Hunt, S. (July 1994). "The Commitment-Trust Theory of Relationship Marketing". The Journal of Marketing. 58 (3): 20–38. doi:10.2307/1252308. JSTOR 1252308.
- Zheng, J.; Roehrich, J.K.; Lewis, M.A. (2008). "The dynamics of contractual and relational governance: Evidence from long-term public-private procurement arrangements". Journal of Purchasing and Supply Management. 14 (1): 43–54. doi:10.1016/j.pursup.2008.01.004. S2CID 207472262.
- Fukuyama, F. (1996) Trust: The Social Virtues and the Creation of Prosperity, Touchstone Books.
- Zak, P. J., and Knack, S. (2001) "Trust and growth". Economic Journal, 111: 295–321.
- Pollitt, Michael (2002). "The economics of trust, norms and networks". Business Ethics: A European Review. 11 (2): 119–128. doi:10.1111/1467-8608.00266. ISSN 1467-8608. S2CID 153788522.
- Braynov, S.; Sandholm, T. (2002). "Contracting With Uncertain Level Of Trust". Computational Intelligence. 18 (4): 501–514. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.70.8413. doi:10.1111/1467-8640.00200. S2CID 33473191.
- Resnick, P. "The value of reputation on eBay: a controlled experiment". Experimental Economics, volume 9, Issue 2, Jun 2006, pp. 79–101.
- Keser, C. (2003) "Experimental games for the design of reputation management systems". IBM Systems J., vol. 42, no. 3.
- Berg, J., Dickhaut, J., and McCabe, K. (1995) "Trust, Reciprocity, and Social History", Games and Economic Behavior 10, 122–142. Abstract.
- Airiau, S., and Sen, S. (2006) "Learning to Commit in Repeated Games". In: Proc. of the Fifth Int. Joint Conf. on Autonomous Agents and Multiagent Systems (AAMAS06).
- Bolton, G. E., Katok, E., and Ockenfels, A. (2003) "How Effective are Electronic Reputation Mechanisms? An Experimental Investigation".
- Camerer, C.; Weigelt, K. (1988). "Experimental Tests of a Sequential Equilibrium Reputation Model". Econometrica. 56 (1): 1–36. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.458.4383. doi:10.2307/1911840. JSTOR 1911840.
- Fehr, E., Kirchsteiger, G., and Riedl, A.. "Does Fairness Prevent Market Clearing? An Experimental Investigation". Quarterly Journal of Economics 108 (May 1993), pp. 437–460.
- Poundstone, W. (1992). Prisoner's Dilemma. Doubleday, NY.
- McKnight, D., H., Chervany, N. L. (2001) "Conceptualizing Trust: A Typology and E-Commerce Customer Relationships Model". Proc. of the 34th Hawaii Int. Conf. on System Sciences.
- Giddens, A. (1991) Modernity and Self-identity: Self and Society in the Late Modern Age. Polity Press.
- Golbeck, J. (2008). Computing with Social Trust. Springer.
- Egger, F. N. "From Interactions to Transactions: Designing the Trust Experience for Business-to-Consumer Electronic Commerce". PhD Thesis, Eindhoven University of Technology (The Netherlands).
- Chang, E., Dillion, T., Hussain, F. K. (2006) Trust and Reputation for Service-Oriented Environments: Technologies for Building Business Intelligence and Consumer Confidence. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
- Fulmer, C. Ashley; Gelfand, Michele J. (2012-07-01). "At What Level (and in Whom) We Trust: Trust Across Multiple Organizational Levels". Journal of Management. 38 (4): 1167–1230. doi:10.1177/0149206312439327. ISSN 0149-2063. S2CID 5506486.
- Dalkir, Kimiz (2017). Knowledge Management in Theory and Practice. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. ISBN 9780262036870.
- Vanhala, Mika; Puumalainen, Kaisu; Blomqvist, Kirsimarja (2011). "Impersonal trust: The development of the construct and the scale". Personnel Review. 40: 485–513. doi:10.1108/00483481111133354.
- Poppo, Laura; Zenger, Todd (2002). "Do formal contracts and relational governance function as substitutes or complements?". Strategic Management Journal. 23 (8): 707–725. doi:10.1002/smj.249. ISSN 0143-2095.
- Lewicki, Roy J.; McAllister, Daniel J.; Bies, Robert J. (1998). "Trust and Distrust: New Relationships and Realities". Academy of Management Review. 23 (3): 438–458. doi:10.2307/259288. ISSN 0363-7425.
- Guo, Shiau-Ling; Lumineau, Fabrice; Lewicki, Roy J. (2017-02-15). "Revisiting the Foundations of Organizational Distrust". Foundations and Trends® in Management. 1 (1): 1–88. doi:10.1561/3400000001. ISSN 2475-6946.
- Tomlinson, Edward; Schnackenberg, Andrew; Dawley, David; Ash, Steven (2020). "Revisiting the trustworthiness-trust relationship: Exploring the differential predictors of cognition- and affect-based trust". Journal of Organizational Behavior. 41 (6): 535–550. doi:10.1002/job.2448. S2CID 218955149.
- McAllister, Daniel (1995). "Affect-and cognition-based trust as foundations for interpersonal cooperation in organizations". Academy of Management Journal. 38 (1): 24–59. doi:10.5465/256727.
- Paulo Verissimo, Miguel Correia, Nuno F. Neves, Paulo Sousa. "Intrusion-Resilient Middleware Design and Validation". In Annals of Emerging Research in Information Assurance, Security and Privacy Services, H. Raghav Rao and Shambhu Upadhyaya (eds.), Elsevier, to appear. 2008.
Further reading
- Bachmann, Reinhard and Zaheer, Akbar (eds) (2006). Handbook of Trust Research. Cheltenham: Edward Elgar.
- Bicchieri, Cristina, Duffy, John and Tolle, Gil (2004). "Trust among strangers", Philosophy of Science 71: 1–34.
- Marková, I., Linell, P & Gillespie, A. (2007). "Trust and distrust in society". In Marková, I. and Gillespie, A. (Eds.) Trust and distrust: Socio-cultural perspectives. Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing, Inc.
- Kelton, Kari; Fleischmann, Kenneth R. & Wallace, William A. (2008). "Trust in Digital Information". Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology, 59(3):363–374.
- Kini, A., & Choobineh, J. (January 1998). "Trust in electronic commerce: Definition and theoretical considerations". Paper presented at the Thirty-First Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences, Kohala Coast, HI.
- Gillespie, A. (2007). "The intersubjective dynamics of trust, distrust and manipulation". In Markova and Gillespie (Eds), Trust & Distrust: Socio-cultural Perspectives. Charlotte, NC: Info Age.
- Maister, David H., Green, Charles H. & Galford, Robert M. (2000). The Trusted Advisor. Free Press, New York
- Schilke, Oliver; Reimann, Martin; Cook, Karen S. (2021). "Trust in Social Relations". Annual Review of Sociology. 47(1).
External links
- "Trust". Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy.
- Trust at PhilPapers
- Trust at the Indiana Philosophy Ontology Project
- Zalta, Edward N. (ed.). "Trust". Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy.
- Trust Building Activities
- Trust: Making and Breaking Cooperative Relations, edited by Diego Gambetta
- Am I Trustworthy? (1950) Educational video clip
- Stony Brook University weekly seminars on the issue of trust in the personal, religious, social, and scientific realms
- World Database of Trust Harvey S. James, Jr., Ph.D. (Updated August 2007) A variety of definitions of trust are collected and listed.