Childhood amnesia

Childhood amnesia, also called infantile amnesia, is the inability of adults to retrieve episodic memories (memories of situations or events) before the age of two to four years. It may also refer to the scarcity or fragmentation of memories recollected from early childhood, particularly occurring between the ages of 2 and 6. On average, this fragmented period wanes off at around 4.7 years.[1][2] Around 5–6 years of age in particular is thought to be when autobiographical memory seems to stabilize and be on par with adults.[3][4] The development of a cognitive self is also thought by some to have an effect on encoding and storing early memories.[5]

Some research has demonstrated that children can remember events from before the age of two, but that these memories may decline as children get older.[6][7][8] Psychologists differ in defining the onset of childhood amnesia. Some define it as the age from which a first memory can be retrieved. This is usually the second birthday, but it can range from two to four years in general.[9][10][11][12]

Changes in encoding, storage and retrieval of memories during early childhood are all important when considering childhood amnesia.[13]

History

Childhood amnesia was first formally reported by psychologist Caroline Miles in her article "A study of individual psychology", in 1895 by the American Journal of Psychology.[14] Five years later, Henri and Henri published a survey showing that most respondents' earliest recollections occurred from the age of two to four.[15] In 1904 G. Stanley Hall noted the phenomenon in his book, Adolescence: Its Psychology and Its Relations to Physiology, Anthropology, Sociology, Sex, Crime, Religion and Education.[16] In 1910, Sigmund Freud offered one of the most famous and controversial descriptions and explanations of childhood amnesia. Using psychoanalytic theory, he postulated that early life events were repressed due to their inappropriately sexual nature. He asserted that childhood or infantile amnesia was a precursor to the 'hysterical amnesia', or repression, presented by his adult patients.[17] Freud asked his patients to recall their earliest memories and found that they had difficulty remembering events from before the age of six to eight.[18] Freud coined the term "infantile" or "childhood amnesia" and discussed this phenomenon in his Three Essays on the Theory of Sexuality.[18] In 1972, Campbell and Spear published a seminal review about childhood amnesia in Psychological Sciences recapping the research conducted to understand this topic from neurological and behavioral perspectives in both human and animal models.

Methods of recall

The method of memory retrieval can influence what can be recalled.[19] Specifically, whether an individual is prompted to remember a specific event, given more general guidelines, or asked to recall any memory possible, the cue method generates different results.

Cued

Many studies use cued recall to retrieve memories.[19] In its basic form, the experimenter gives the participant a word, and the participant responds with the first memory they think of associated with that word.[15] This method has generally estimated the age of offset at about three to five but can vary. There are several objections to the cue method. One memory is recorded per cue word, so it can be difficult to know whether this memory is their earliest memory or the first memory that popped to mind. It may be a problem if participants are not asked to record the earliest memory they can recall which relates to the cue. If the experimenter asks the participant to specifically use childhood memories or the earliest memories associated with a cue, the age estimate can be two to eight years.[15][20][21] Even with this measure, cued recall is only useful for bringing to mind memories formed several months after the introduction of that word into the participant's vocabulary.[22] One study performed by Bauer and Larkina (2013) used cued recall by asking children and adults to state a personal memory related to the word and then state the earliest time that it occurred. The researchers found that the younger children need more prompts or cues. For children and adults, the earliest memory retrieval was around three years old.[12]

Free

Free recall refers to the specific paradigm in the psychological study of memory where participants study a list of items on a specific trial and are then prompted to recall the items in any order. Free recall, in regard to childhood amnesia, is the process by which experimenters ask individuals for their earliest memories, and allow the participant to respond freely.[15] There is no significant difference when people are instructed to recall their earliest memories with cued recall compared to free recall. It is thought that a major benefit of free recall is that every question gets answered which may, in turn, elicit memories from an earlier age.[15]

Exhaustive

In the exhaustive recall method, participants are asked to record all the memories they can access before a specific age.[15] This method, like free recall, relies on participants to come up with memories without cues. Exhaustive recall yields a better understanding than others on the number of memories surviving from early childhood but can be demanding for the subjects who often have to spend many hours trying to remember events from their childhood.[15] No major differences among word cued, interview, focused and exhaustive recall have been found.[23]

Accessible and inaccessible memories

The number of early childhood memories a person can recall depends on many factors, including the emotion associated with the event, their age at the time of the remembered event and the age at the time they are asked to recall an early memory.[8] Although people assume that not recalling a childhood memory means they have forgotten the event, there is a difference between availability and accessibility. The availability of memory is the idea that the memory is intact and is in-memory storage. Accessibility of memory is dictated by the moment in time that a person attempts to recall that memory. Therefore, cues may influence which memories are accessible at any given time even though there may be many more available memories that are not accessed.[15] Some other research suggests that people's earliest memories date back to the ages of 3 or 4 years. Usher and Neisser reported that some events, like the birth of a sibling and a planned hospitalization, can be readily remembered if they occurred at age 2.[20] The bits and pieces of such memories that were obtained in their research may not be indicative of genuine episodic memory. An alternative hypothesis is that these apparent memories are the result of educated guesses, general knowledge of what must have been, or external information acquired after the age of 2.[24]

According to a study by West and Bauer, their research suggests that earlier memories have less emotional content than later memories, and are less personally meaningful, unique, or intense. Earlier memories also do not seem to differ greatly in perspective.[15][25][26] Certain life events do result in clearer and earlier memories. Adults find it easier to remember personal, rather than public, event memories from early childhood. This means a person would remember getting a dog, but not the appearance of Halley's Comet. Psychologists have debated the age of adults' earliest memories. Most modern data suggests somewhere between the ages 2 and 4 on average. Some research shows that the offset of childhood amnesia (earliest age of recall) is 2 years of age for hospitalization and sibling birth and 3 years of age for death or change in houses. Thus, some memories are available from earlier in childhood than previous research has suggested.[20]

Some research suggests that until around the age of 4, children cannot form context-rich memories. Although more evidence is needed, the relative lack of episodic memories of early childhood may be linked to maturation of prefrontal cortex.[27] It also suggest adults can access fragment memories (isolated moments without context, often remembered as images, behaviors, or emotions) from around age 3, whereas event memories are usually recalled from slightly later.[20][28][15] This is similar to research showing the difference between personal recollections and known events. Known memories change to more personal recollections at approximately 4 34 years of age.

Fading memories

Children can form memories at younger ages than adults can recall. While the efficiency of encoding and storage processes allows older children to remember more,[29] younger children also have great memory capacity. Infants can remember the actions of sequences, the objects used to produce them, and the order in which the actions unfold, suggesting that they possess the precursors necessary for autobiographical memory.[14] Children's recall is 50% accurate for events that happened before the age of two[7] whereas adults remember near to nothing before that age.[6] By age two, children can retrieve memories after several weeks, indicating that these memories could become relatively enduring and could explain why some people have memories from this young.[6][14] Children also show an ability to nonverbally recall events that occurred before they had the vocabulary to describe them, whereas adults do not.[22][30][31] Findings such as these prompted research into when and why people lose these previously accessible memories.

Some suggest that as children age, they lose the ability to recall preverbal memories.[32] One explanation for this maintains that after developing linguistic skills, memories that were not encoded verbally get lost within the mind.[32] This theory also explains why many individuals' early memories are fragmented – the nonverbal components were lost.[33] Contrary findings indicate that elementary aged children remember a greater amount of accurate details about events than they had reported at a younger age[34] and that 6- to 9-year-old children tend to have verbally accessible memories from very early childhood.[34] Research on animal models seems to indicate that childhood amnesia is not only due to the development of language or any other human proper faculty.

This increased ability for children to remember their early years does not start to fade until children reach double digits.[33] By the age of eleven, children exhibit young adult levels of childhood amnesia.[33] These findings may indicate that there is some aspect of the adolescent brain, or the neurobiological processes of adolescence, that prompts the development of childhood amnesia.

Animal models

The phenomenon of infantile amnesia is not specific to humans. This initially was researched in rat models and found that younger rats forget a conditioned avoidance response to a shock-paired compartment faster than older rats did. These findings have also been replicated in a number of different species with different learning paradigms.[35] The importance of animal model research should not be understated as these studies have informed neurobiological findings about childhood amnesia and would be impossible to ethically conduct in humans.

Because infantile amnesia has been observed in animals, the occurrence cannot be explained just by cognition specific to humans such as reading and writing or an understanding of self. Of course, a main criticism of animal models is that development and cognition in animals and humans are starkly different. Researchers have attempted to address this by creating timelines for animal development based on changes in learning and memory abilities, brain development, and hormones.[35][36]

The research done in rat models indicates that failures in memory retrieval are to blame for infantile amnesia. In a study on rats, it was found that one single "reminder" before a test was enough to reduce forgetting in infant rats. A similar study was done on infants, showing that a behavior that was typically forgotten in a few days could be remembered if the subject was exposed to the reinforcer before a test. These studies indicate that these memories are not being completely lost, but are simply not being retrieved.[37]

Individual differences

Many factors affect memory in humans, including gender and culture.[38][39] Differences in early memory between these groups can tell us about potential causes and implications of childhood amnesia. Importantly, the individual differences described below tell us that elaborative parenting styles and emphasis of cultural history when teaching children may result in recollection of earlier childhood memories. This suggests that childhood amnesia offset is modifiable by parenting and teaching styles and is therefore not only predetermined or completely biological.

Sex

Generally, when a sex discrepancy is found in the age at first memories, females have earlier memories than males.[40] Women's earlier first memories may be accounted for by the fact that mothers generally have more elaborative, evaluative, and emotional reminiscent style with daughters than with sons,[41] which has been shown to result in more richly detailed childhood memories.[41] Women across cultures tend to have more information-dense memories than men,[40] and women tend to refer to others more than themselves in their earliest memories.[40][42] Men, on the other hand, exhibit more early memory focus on their individual selves. Men have been found more likely than women to mention negative memories.[40] Contrarily, studies have shown that girls are more likely to remember traumatic and transitional events, whereas boys more often remember play events.[33] Early recollections have also been found more accurate in their reflections of friendliness for men and dominance for women.[43]

Ethnicity, culture, and society

MacDonald et al. found that Chinese participants had later first memories than New Zealand European (Pakeha) or Maori participants. This effect was due to Chinese females, whose average age at first memory was 6.1 years.[40] This indicates that Chinese women have later first memories than Chinese men, which differs from the general finding that women report earlier first memories than men. It has been suggested that since sons are prized far over daughters in China, parents may have more elaborate, evaluative, and emotional reminiscent styles with boys than with girls.[40] Among American subjects, it has been found that Black women have later memories than Black males or White females.[42] Black women also tend to report a low proportion of personal experience[42] which is independently correlated with being older at the age of first memory. It may be that White parents are more likely to use directives to elaborately reminisce than Black parents are with daughters in Black American culture.[42]

Findings that Korean individuals have significantly later first memories than American individuals were originally thought to be caused by the collectivist nature of Asian cultures.[40] The lack of an age discrepancy between Chinese males and New Zealand European individuals serves to cast doubt on this theory.[40] Additionally, studies on the Black American population, which is considered a more collectivist society, have not indicated later first memories than non-collectivist cultures.[42] It has been shown that children from Western cultures tell more elaborated, detailed, and emotional narratives than children from Eastern cultures.[41]

Maori adults report significantly earlier memories than either Pakeha or Chinese individuals. The traditional emphasis on the past in Maori culture may have led to an earlier understanding of the nature of time, and to the retrieval of earlier memories. Maori are also more likely than Pakeha or Chinese individuals to indicate a family story as a source for their memory.[40]

Personality

Individuals' first memories significantly reflect their personality traits.[44] People who reveal a more detailed memory are more likely to be open in their daily lives and disclose a range of personal information to others.[43] Characteristics of early recollections are reflective of friendliness for males and dominance for females.[43]

Forgetfulness

Even when childhood events are not remembered episodically, they can be implicitly remembered. Humans can be primed and implicitly trained earlier before they can remember facts or autobiographical events.[27] Adults can generally recall events from 3–4 years old,[9][12] with those that have primarily experiential memories beginning around 4.7 years old. Adults who experienced traumatic or abusive early childhoods report a longer period of childhood amnesia, ending around 5–7 years old.[9] One possible cause for this is stress-related injury to the brain, as trauma damages memory centers and negatively impacts recall.[9][45]

This evidence for delayed and impaired recall of trauma due to the trauma itself is contrary to the concept of 'repressed' memories, wherein traumatic memories are stored, intact, in the memory in order to 'protect' the subject and are able to be 'recovered' with full accompanying narrative.[46] Since priming can occur at a younger age than episodic recall, children in abusive situations may form implicit memory connections of violence even when no true episodic recall exists.[27]

Falsity

Very few adults have memories from before 2.5 years old. Those who do report memories from before this age usually cannot tell the difference between personal memory of the event and simple knowledge of it, which may have come from other sources.[21] Events from after the age of 10 years are relatively easy to remember correctly, whereas memories from the age of 2 are more often confused with false images and memories.[47] Memories from early childhood (around age two) are susceptible to false suggestion, making them less trustworthy. These should be treated with caution, especially if they have severe consequences.[47] Imagining details of a false event can encourage the generation of false memories.[47] Studies have shown that people who merely imagine a childhood event are more likely to believe that it happened to them compared to events they did not imagine.[48] This term has been coined imagination inflation and shows that merely imagining an event can make it seem more plausible that it really happened. Using the same paradigm, people that are shown a doctored photograph of themselves as a child in an event that never occurred can create false memories of the event by imagining the event over time.[49] Therefore, this implies that it would be possible for false memories to be generated in and/or fed by a court case. This concern has led the APA to advise caution in accepting memories of physically and sexually abusive events from before the age of two. They also recommend that these memories not be entirely discounted, due to the heinous nature of the crimes.[2]

Maternal Narrative Style

Researchers who have used maternal narrative style as a predictor for early childhood memory recall have found that children whose mothers frequently and elaborately reminisced events with the child were able to recall earlier first memories.[50] Studies also show that when mothers include emotional context when reminiscing with their children, the child is likely to grow up with a more secure attachment style, as well as show higher levels of emotional understanding and a stronger sense of self.[51] These factors all contribute to the child developing stronger autobiographical memory skills.[51]

Proposed explanations

Freud's trauma theory

Sigmund Freud is famous for his theories of psychosexual development which suggest that people's personality traits stem from their libido (sexual appetite) which develops from early childhood experiences.[17] Freud's trauma theory, originally named "Seduction Theory" posits that childhood amnesia was the result of the mind's attempt to repress memories of traumatic events (i.e. sexual abuse by caretakers) that occurred in the psychosexual development of every child. This supposedly led to the repression of the majority of memories from the first years of life when children were supposedly obsessed with exploring their sexuality.[17] Notably, Freud himself abandoned this theory in the late 1800s.[52] Freudian theory, including his explanation for childhood amnesia, has been criticized for extensive use of anecdotal evidence rather than scientific research, and his observations that allow for multiple interpretations.[53]

While the Freudian psychosexual theory is debated, there are some insights to be made into the effect of childhood emotional abuse on memory. Examining the effects of emotional trauma and childhood amnesia shows that stressful experiences do in fact disrupt memory and can damage central parts of the memory system such as the hippocampus and amygdala.[9] Adults who were abused or traumatized in childhood form their earliest memories about 2–3 years after the general population. In addition, they demonstrate considerable problems with visual, pictorial, and facial memory storage and retrieval compared to non-traumatized individuals.[9] This implies that trauma can disrupt the formation of early childhood memories, but does not necessarily give evidence for Freud's theory of repression.

Emotion

The amygdala (which is primarily concerned with emotions and emotional content of memories) and the hippocampus (which concerns primarily autobiographical memories) are generally independent, but emotions and the amygdala are known to play a role in memory encoding, which is typically associated with the hippocampus.[54] Research has found that later memories in childhood have more propositional and emotional content than earlier memories and are rated as more meaningful and vivid. It has been suggested that differences in the emotions experienced by infants and adults may be a cause of childhood amnesia.[25] Whether highly emotional events can stimulate and improve reliable recall (flashbulb memories) is still highly debated.[55]

Some studies have discovered that emotional experiences are connected with faster retrieval times, leading to the belief that emotional events have heightened accessibility in our memories.[2] If an event is particularly surprising, it receives prioritized processing in the brain, most likely due to evolutionary reasons. The evolutionary psychology states that if a past event was particularly frightening or upsetting, one is apt to avoid a similar situation in the future, especially if it is endangering to one's well-being. In addition, the more significant an event, the bigger impact it has and the more rehearsal it receives.[2]

Various findings have shown that events such as hospitalization and the birth of a sibling are correlated with an earlier offset of childhood amnesia, which may be because they were more emotionally memorable.[20] Other seemingly emotional memories such as the death of a family member or having to move do not affect offset, possibly because the events were not as meaningful to the child.[20] Some memories are therefore available from earlier in childhood than others, which has led to the conclusion that very emotional events can be encoded and recalled earlier than non-emotional events.[20]

Neurology

One possible explanation for childhood amnesia is the lack of neurological development of the infant brain, preventing the creation of long term or autobiographical memories.[2] The hippocampus and prefrontal cortex, two key structures in the neuroanatomy of memory, do not develop into mature structures until around the age of three or four. These structures are known to be associated with the formation of autobiographical memories.[27][53] Specifically, the dentate gyrus, a section of the hippocampus, has been observed to primarily develop in the postnatal stages of development. It is theorized that this part of the hippocampus plays a large role in the ability to form and retrieve memories. The ability to store and recall memories gradually increases along with the postnatal development of the dentate gyrus.[56]

The physiological approach appears to support findings of memory loss in relation to amnesiacs and others who have experienced damage to the hippocampus. They cannot efficiently store or recall memories from past events, but still exhibit perceptual and cognitive skills and can still learn new information.[2] The development of the Medial Temporal Lobe (MTL), which contains the hippocampus, has been found to specifically have a defining impact on the ability to encode and maintain memories from early childhood.[57]

While the neurological explanation does account for blanks in very young children's memories, it does not give a full explanation for childhood amnesia because it fails to account for the years after the age of four. It also fails to address the issue that children themselves do not show childhood amnesia.[2] Children around the age of two to three have been found to remember things that occurred when they were only one to two years old.[31] This discovery that three-year-olds can retrieve memories from earlier in their life implies that all necessary neurological structures are in place to recall episodic information over the short-term, but evidently not over the long-term into adulthood.[58] The finding that all altricial species experience profound forgetting of episodic information formed during infancy suggests that human-centric explanations of infantile amnesia are inherently incomplete. A comprehensive understanding of infantile amnesia will require a neurobiological explanation of why infants forget.

There are reasons to believe that different associations within the cerebral hemisphere have an effect on remembering events from a very early period in a person's life. Mixed-handedness and bilateral saccadic eye movements (as opposed to vertical or pursuit eye movements) have been associated with an earlier offset of childhood amnesia, leading to the conclusion that interactions between the two hemispheres correlate with increased memory for early childhood events.[59]

Neurobiology

Research into the neural substrates of infantile amnesia using animal models has found that the major inhibitory neurotransmitter gamma-amino butyric acid (GABA) may be involved in the regulation of retrieval of infantile memories in adults.[60] GABA activity is known to be higher in early childhood development than it is in adulthood, both in humans and other animals. Researchers have hypothesized that increased GABA activity in development has an effect on memory retrieval later in life. Past studies have shown that GABA aids in the forgetting of fear memories in infancy and that it may be a general mechanism for regulating infant memory retrieval.[61] Benzodiazepines, a class of psychiatric medication which increase GABA expression, have been found to produce anterograde amnesia, or a failure to encode memories after taking the medication. Subjects taking benzodiazepines are found to perform worse on learning and memory tasks compared to drug-naïve subjects.[62]

Previously, it was assumed that neurogenesis, or the continued production of neurons, ended after development. Recent findings have shown that there are high levels of neurogenesis in the hippocampus in early childhood which taper out into adulthood, although neurogenesis continues to persist slowly.[63] As the hippocampus is known to be vital to memory processes, there are obvious implications for childhood amnesia. Animal research has shown that the age of high neurogenesis is in the developmental period when persistent memories are least likely to be formed. It has been proposed that hippocampal neurogenesis degrades existing memories. This may be due to increased competition between the new and existing neurons, followed by the replacement of synapses in preexisting memory circuits. This theory has been supported in mouse models in which increasing neurogenesis levels also increased forgetting.[64][65] Additionally, decreasing neurogenesis after new memory formation resulted in decreased forgetting.[65] Are 'lost' infant memories permanently erased (i.e., storage failure) or do they become progressively inaccessible with time (i.e., retrieval failure)? Consistent with a deficit in memory retrieval, optogenetic reactivation of neuronal ensembles that encoded the memory drive memory recall in adulthood.[66] In addition, memories are consolidated via transferral from the hippocampus to the cortex. This transferral occurs preferably during periods of elevated excitability in the hippocampus i.e. during ripple oscillations. Ripple oscillations represent increased hippocampo-cortical communication. This increase in experience-associated activity does not occur up to a certain age suggesting that this might be a mechanism for infantile amnesia.[67]

By stimulating neurons used in 'forgotten' fear conditioning in infant mice, Guskjolen et al. found retrieval of the fear response was possible by optogenetic reactivation of the neuronal ensembles that encoded the memory drive. This retrieval lasted for up to three months, suggesting the infantile amnesia was underlaid by a biological failure to access, rather than encode, said memories.[68]

In addition, memories are consolidated via transferral from the hippocampus to the cortex. This transferral occurs preferably during periods of elevated activity in the hippocampus. This increase in experience-associated activity does not occur up to a certain age, suggesting that this inability to transfer information might be a mechanism for infantile amnesia.[67]

Development

The development of a cognitive self is also thought by some to have a strong effect on encoding and storing early memories.[2] As toddlers grow, a developing sense of the self begins to emerge as they realize that they are a person with unique and defining characteristics and have individual thoughts and feelings separate from others. As they gain a sense of the self, they can begin to organize autobiographical experiences and retain memories of past events. This is also known as the development of a theory of mind which refers to a child's acceptance that they have beliefs, knowledge, and thoughts that no one else has access to.[2]

The developmental explanation asserts that young children have a good concept of semantic information but lack the retrieval processes necessary to link past and present episodic events to create an autobiographical self.[27] Young children do not seem to have a sense of a continuous self over time until they develop awareness for themselves as an individual human being. Some research suggests this awareness is thought to form around the age of 4 or 5, as children in this time period can understand that recent past events affect the present, while 3-year-old children still seem unable to grasp this concept.[69]

This acknowledged link of the past to the present and the concept of continuous-time and therefore a continuous self is also helped by memory talk with adults.[2] Through elaboration and repetition of events experienced, adults help children to encode memories as a part of their personal past and it becomes essential to their being.[2]

Language

The incomplete development of language in young children is thought to be a critical cause of childhood amnesia as infants do not yet have the language capacity necessary to encode autobiographical memories.[2] The typical schedule of language development seems to support this theory .[53] There appears to be a direct correlation between the development of language in children, and the earliest age at which they can obtain childhood memories (around the age 3–4).[53] Performance on both verbal and nonverbal memory tasks shows that children with more advanced language abilities can report more during a verbal interview and exhibit superior nonverbal memory compared to children with less advanced language skills.[30] If children lack language, they are unable to describe memories from infancy because they do not have the words and knowledge to explain them. Adults and children can often remember memories from around three or four years of age which is during a time of rapid language development. Before language develops, children often only hold preverbal memories and may use symbols to represent them. Therefore, once language develops, one can actively describe their memories with words. The context that one is in when they encode or retrieve memories is different for adults and infants because language is not present during infancy.[32]

Language allows children to organize personal past and present experiences and share these memories with others.[2] This exchange of dialogue makes children aware of their personal past and encourages them to think about their cognitive self and how past activities have affected them in the present.[41] Several studies have shown that simply discussing events with children, in general, could lead to more easily retrievable memories.[6] There has also been researching that suggests the degree to which a child discusses events with adults shapes autobiographical recollection.[70] This has implications for gender and cultural differences. Autobiographical memory begins to emerge as parents engage in memory talk with their children and encourage them to think about why a certain event happened. Memory talk allows children to develop memory systems in order to categorize generic versus unique events.[2]

The social-cultural developmental perspective states that both language and culture play a role in the development of a child's autobiographical memory.[71] An important aspect of this theory considers the difference between parents who discuss memories at length with their children in an elaborative style, and those who do not. Children of parents who discuss memories with them in an elaborative style report a greater number of memories than children who do not discuss their memories. Memories are described in greater detail. This has implications for cultural differences.

See also

References

  1. "Childhood Amnesia - an overview | ScienceDirect Topics". www.sciencedirect.com. Retrieved 2023-03-04.
  2. Robinson-Riegler B, Robinson-Riegler G (2012). Cognitive Psychology: Applying the Science of the Mind (Third ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson Education Inc. as Allyn & Bacon. pp. 272–276, 295–296, 339–346. ISBN 978-0-205-17674-8.
  3. Nelson, K.; Fivush, R. (2004). "The emergence of autobiographical memory: A social cultural developmental theory". Psychological Review. 111 (2): 486–511. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.111.2.486. PMID 15065919. S2CID 18912310.
  4. Hayne, Harlene; Jack, Fiona (2011). "Childhood amnesia". WIREs Cognitive Science. 2 (2): 136–145. doi:10.1002/wcs.107. PMID 26302005.
  5. Howe ML (31 March 2015). "Memory Development". In Lerner RM, Overton WF, Molenaar PC (eds.). Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science, Cognitive Processes. John Wiley & Sons. ISBN 9781118953846 via Google Books.
  6. Fivush R, Schwarzmueller A (1999). "Children remember childhood: implications for childhood amnesia". Applied Cognitive Psychology. 12 (5): 455–473. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(199810)12:5<455::AID-ACP534>3.0.CO;2-H.
  7. Clevelend E, Reese E (2008). "Children remember early childhood: Long-term recall across the offset of childhood amnesia". Applied Cognitive Psychology. 22 (1): 127–142. doi:10.1002/acp.1359.
  8. Tustin K, Hayne H (September 2010). "Defining the boundary: age-related changes in childhood amnesia". Developmental Psychology. 46 (5): 1049–1061. doi:10.1037/a0020105. PMID 20822222.
  9. Joseph R (2003). "Emotional Trauma and Childhood Amnesia". Consciousness & Emotion. 4 (2): 151–179. doi:10.1075/ce.4.2.02jos.
  10. Eacott MJ (April 1999). "Memory for the Events of Early Childhood" (PDF). Current Directions in Psychological Science. 8 (2): 46–48. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00011. S2CID 17155352. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2015-11-06. Retrieved 2016-02-17.
  11. Fiona J, Harlene H (18 July 2007). "Eliciting adults' earliest memories: Does it matter how we ask the question?". Memory. 15 (6): 647–663. doi:10.1080/09658210701467087. PMID 17654279. S2CID 5775522.
  12. Bauer PJ, Larkina M (April 2014). "Childhood amnesia in the making: different distributions of autobiographical memories in children and adults". Journal of Experimental Psychology. General. 143 (2): 597–611. doi:10.1037/a0033307. PMID 23937179.
  13. Hayne H (2004). "Infant memory development: Implications for childhood amnesia". Developmental Review. 24: 33–73. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.132.3284. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2003.09.007.
  14. Bauer P (2004). "Oh where, oh where have those early memories gone? A developmental perspective on childhood amnesia". Psychological Science Agenda. 18 (12).
  15. Jack F, Hayne H (August 2007). "Eliciting adults' earliest memories: does it matter how we ask the question?". Memory. 15 (6): 647–663. doi:10.1080/09658210701467087. PMID 17654279. S2CID 5775522.
  16. Hall GS (1904). Adolescence. New York: D. Appleton and Company. ISBN 978-0-13-008631-0.
  17. Freud S (1910). "II". Three Contributions to the Sexual Theory. New York: The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease Publishing Company.
  18. Freud S (1953). Three essays on the theory of sexuality. London: Hogarth Press.
  19. Bauer PJ, Burch MM, Scholin SE, Güler OE (October 2007). "Using cue words to investigate the distribution of autobiographical memories in childhood". Psychological Science. 18 (10): 910–916. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01999.x. PMID 17894609. S2CID 34100638.
  20. Usher JA, Neisser U (June 1993). "Childhood amnesia and the beginnings of memory for four early life events". Journal of Experimental Psychology. General. 122 (2): 155–165. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.122.2.155. PMID 8315398.
  21. Eacott MJ, Crawley RA (March 1998). "The offset of childhood amnesia: memory for events that occurred before age 3". Journal of Experimental Psychology. General. 127 (1): 22–33. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.127.1.22. PMID 9503650.
  22. Morrison CM, Conway MA (July 2010). "First words and first memories". Cognition. 116 (1): 23–32. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.03.011. PMID 20363469. S2CID 6068018.
  23. Rubin DC (July 2000). "The distribution of early childhood memories". Memory. 8 (4): 265–269. doi:10.1080/096582100406810. hdl:10161/10139. PMID 10932795. S2CID 1259926.
  24. Loftus EF (June 1993). "Desperately seeking memories of the first few years of childhood: the reality of early memories". Journal of Experimental Psychology. General. 122 (2): 274–277. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.122.2.274. PMID 8315402.
  25. West TA, Bauer PJ (May 1999). "Assumptions of infantile amnesia: are there differences between early and later memories?". Memory. 7 (3): 257–278. doi:10.1080/096582199387913. PMID 10659077.
  26. Fivush R, Gray JT, Fromhoff FA (1987). "Two-year-olds talk about the past". Cognitive Development. 2 (4): 393–409. doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(87)80015-1.
  27. Newcombe N, Drummey A, Fox N, Lai E, Ottinger-Alberts W (2000). "embering Early Childhood: How Much, How, and Why (or Why Not)". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 9 (2): 55–58. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.417.7509. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00060. S2CID 18780584.
  28. Bruce D, Wilcox-O'Hearn LA, Robinson JA, Phillips-Grant K, Francis L, Smith MC (June 2005). "Fragment memories mark the end of childhood amnesia". Memory & Cognition. 33 (4): 567–576. doi:10.3758/bf03195324. PMID 16248322.
  29. Bauer PJ (April 2006). "Constructing a past in infancy: a neuro-developmental account". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 10 (4): 175–181. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.02.009. PMID 16537115. S2CID 18845770.
  30. Simcock G, Hayne H (September 2003). "Age-related changes in verbal and nonverbal memory during early childhood". Developmental Psychology. 39 (5): 805–814. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.39.5.805. PMID 12952395.
  31. Bauer P, Wenner J, Dropik P, Wewerka S (August 2000). "Parameters of remembering and forgetting in the transition from infancy to early childhood". Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 65 (4): v–204.
  32. Simcock G, Hayne H (May 2002). "Breaking the barrier? Children fail to translate their preverbal memories into language". Psychological Science. 13 (3): 225–231. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00442. PMID 12009042. S2CID 40069463.
  33. Peterson C, Grant VV, Boland LD (August 2005). "Childhood amnesia in children and adolescents: their earliest memories". Memory. 13 (6): 622–637. doi:10.1080/09658210444000278. PMID 16076676. S2CID 28074951.
  34. Van Abbema DL, Bauer PJ (November 2005). "Autobiographical memory in middle childhood: recollections of the recent and distant past". Memory. 13 (8): 829–845. doi:10.1080/09658210444000430. PMID 16298891. S2CID 41414931.
  35. Feigley DA, Spear NE (December 1970). "Effect of age and punishment condition on long-term retention by the rat of active- and passive-avoidance learning". Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 73 (3): 515–526. doi:10.1037/h0030234. PMID 5514687.
  36. Greco C, Rovee-Collier C, Hayne H, Griesler P, Earley L (1986-10-01). "Ontogeny of early event memory: I. Forgetting and retrieval by 2- and 3-month-olds". Infant Behavior and Development. 9 (4): 441–460. doi:10.1016/0163-6383(86)90017-2.
  37. Madsen, Heather Bronwyn; Kim, Jee Hyun (February 2016). "Ontogeny of memory: An update on 40 years of work on infantile amnesia". Behavioural Brain Research. 298 (Pt A): 4–14. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2015.07.030. ISSN 0166-4328. PMID 26190765. S2CID 30878407.
  38. Kingo OS, Berntsen D, Krøjgaard P (September 2013). "Adults' earliest memories as a function of age, gender, and education in a large stratified sample" (PDF). Psychology and Aging. 28 (3): 646–653. doi:10.1037/a0031356. PMID 23421324.
  39. Gutchess AH, Indeck A (2009-01-01). "Cultural influences on memory". In Chiao JY (ed.). Cultural Neuroscience: Cultural Influences on Brain Function. Progress in Brain Research. Vol. 178. pp. 137–150. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.493.6194. doi:10.1016/s0079-6123(09)17809-3. ISBN 9780444533616. PMID 19874966.
  40. MacDonald S, Uesiliana K, Hayne H (November 2000). "Cross-cultural and gender differences in childhood amnesia" (PDF). Memory. 8 (6): 365–376. doi:10.1080/09658210050156822. PMID 11145068. S2CID 25373074. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2010-05-25. Retrieved 2009-09-14.
  41. Fivush R, Nelson K (September 2004). "Culture and language in the emergence of autobiographical memory". Psychological Science. 15 (9): 573–577. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00722.x. PMID 15327626. S2CID 12384439.
  42. Fitzgerald JM (September 2010). "Culture, gender, and the first memories of black and white American students". Memory & Cognition. 38 (6): 785–796. doi:10.3758/MC.38.6.785. PMID 20852241.
  43. Barrett D (1983). "Early Recollections as Predictors of Self Disclosure and Interpersonal Style". Journal of Individual Psychology. 39: 92–98.
  44. Barrett D (1980). "The first memory as a predictor of personality traits". Journal of Individual Psychology. 36 (2): 136–149.
  45. Bremner JD (December 2006). "Traumatic stress: effects on the brain". Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience. 8 (4): 445–461. doi:10.31887/DCNS.2006.8.4/jbremner. PMC 3181836. PMID 17290802.
  46. Otgaar H, Howe ML, Patihis L, Merckelbach H, Lynn SJ, Lilienfeld SO, Loftus EF (November 2019). "The Return of the Repressed: The Persistent and Problematic Claims of Long-Forgotten Trauma". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 14 (6): 1072–1095. doi:10.1177/1745691619862306. PMC 6826861. PMID 31584864.
  47. Strange D, Wade K, Hayne H (2008). "Creating false memories for events that occurred before versus after the offset of childhood amnesia". Memory. 16 (5): 475–484. doi:10.1080/09658210802059049. PMID 18569677. S2CID 43084237.
  48. Garry M, Manning CG, Loftus EF, Sherman SJ (June 1996). "Imagination inflation: Imagining a childhood event inflates confidence that it occurred". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 3 (2): 208–214. doi:10.3758/BF03212420. PMID 24213869.
  49. Wade KA, Garry M, Read JD, Lindsay DS (September 2002). "A picture is worth a thousand lies: using false photographs to create false childhood memories". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 9 (3): 597–603. doi:10.3758/BF03196318. PMID 12412902.
  50. Wang, Qi; Gülgöz, Sami (2020-05-21), "Introduction: New perspectives on childhood memory: introduction to the special issue", Remembering and Forgetting Early Childhood, Routledge, pp. 1–5, doi:10.4324/9781003030102-1, ISBN 978-1-003-03010-2, S2CID 219501051, retrieved 2022-06-07
  51. Fivush, Robyn (2006-11-28). "Maternal Reminiscing Style and Children's Developing Understanding of Self and Emotion". Clinical Social Work Journal. 35 (1): 37–46. doi:10.1007/s10615-006-0065-1. ISSN 0091-1674. S2CID 145333318.
  52. Israëls H, Schatzman M (March 1993). "The seduction theory". History of Psychiatry. 4 (13): 23–59. doi:10.1177/0957154x9300401302. PMID 11612951. S2CID 5158634.
  53. Gleitman H, Fridlund A, Reisberg D (2007). Psychology (7 ed.). New York: W. W. Norton & Company. ISBN 978-0-393-97768-4.
  54. Phelps EA (April 2004). "Human emotion and memory: interactions of the amygdala and hippocampal complex". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 14 (2): 198–202. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.015. PMID 15082325. S2CID 13888599.
  55. McCloskey M, Wible C, Cohen N (1988). "Is there a special Flashbulb-Memory mechanism?" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 117 (2): 171–181. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.117.2.171. ISSN 0096-3445. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2011-07-20.
  56. Josselyn SA, Frankland PW (August 2012). "Infantile amnesia: a neurogenic hypothesis". Learning & Memory. 19 (9): 423–433. doi:10.1101/lm.021311.110. PMID 22904373.
  57. Squire LR, Stark CE, Clark RE (2004). "The medial temporal lobe". Annual Review of Neuroscience. 27: 279–306. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144130. PMID 15217334.
  58. Bauer P (June 2007). "Recall in infancy: A neurodevelopmental account". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 16 (3): 142–146. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00492.x. S2CID 145381061.
  59. Christman SD, Propper RE, Brown TJ (May 2006). "Increased interhemispheric interaction is associated with earlier offset of childhood amnesia". Neuropsychology. 20 (3): 336–345. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.20.3.336. PMID 16719626.
  60. Madsen HB, Kim JH (February 2016). "Ontogeny of memory: An update on 40 years of work on infantile amnesia". Behavioural Brain Research. 298 (Pt A): 4–14. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2015.07.030. PMID 26190765. S2CID 30878407.
  61. Kim JH, Richardson R (December 2007). "Immediate post-reminder injection of gamma-amino butyric acid (GABA) agonist midazolam attenuates reactivation of forgotten fear in the infant rat". Behavioral Neuroscience. 121 (6): 1328–1332. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.560.7099. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.121.6.1328. PMID 18085885.
  62. Roth T, Roehrs T, Wittig R, Zorick F (1984). "Benzodiazepines and memory". British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology. 18 (Suppl 1): 45S–49S. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2125.1984.tb02581.x. PMC 1463341. PMID 6151849.
  63. Ming GL, Song H (2005-01-01). "Adult neurogenesis in the mammalian central nervous system". Annual Review of Neuroscience. 28: 223–250. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.051804.101459. PMID 16022595.
  64. Josselyn SA, Frankland PW (August 2012). "Infantile amnesia: a neurogenic hypothesis". Learning & Memory. 19 (9): 423–433. doi:10.1101/lm.021311.110. PMID 22904373.
  65. Akers KG, Martinez-Canabal A, Restivo L, Yiu AP, De Cristofaro A, Hsiang HL, et al. (May 2014). "Hippocampal neurogenesis regulates forgetting during adulthood and infancy". Science. 344 (6184): 598–602. Bibcode:2014Sci...344..598A. doi:10.1126/science.1248903. PMID 24812394. S2CID 3616561.
  66. Guskjolen, A., Kenney, J. W., de la Parra, J., Yeung, B. R. A., Josselyn, S. A., & Frankland, P. W. (2018). Recovery of “lost” infant memories in mice. Current Biology, 28(14), 2283-2290. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.05.059
  67. Farooq U, Dragoi G (January 2019). "Emergence of preconfigured and plastic time-compressed sequences in early postnatal development". Science. 363 (6423): 168–173. Bibcode:2019Sci...363..168F. doi:10.1126/science.aav0502. PMC 6794005. PMID 30630930.
  68. Guskjolen A, Kenney JW, de la Parra J, Yeung BA, Josselyn SA, Frankland PW (July 2018). "Recovery of "Lost" Infant Memories in Mice". Current Biology. 28 (14): 2283–2290.e3. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2018.05.059. PMID 29983316. S2CID 51602223.
  69. Povinelli DJ, Landry AM, Theall LA, Clark BR, Castille CM (November 1999). "Development of young children's understanding that the recent past is causally bound to the present". Developmental Psychology. 35 (6): 1426–1439. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.35.6.1426. PMID 10563732.
  70. Mullen MK (July 1994). "Earliest recollections of childhood: a demographic analysis". Cognition. 52 (1): 55–79. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(94)90004-3. PMID 7924199. S2CID 40892911.
  71. Nelson K, Fivush R (April 2004). "The emergence of autobiographical memory: a social cultural developmental theory". Psychological Review. 111 (2): 486–511. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.335.6967. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.111.2.486. PMID 15065919. S2CID 18912310.

Further reading

This article is issued from Wikipedia. The text is licensed under Creative Commons - Attribution - Sharealike. Additional terms may apply for the media files.