Persistent vegetative state
A persistent vegetative state (PVS) or post-coma unresponsiveness[1] (PCU) is a disorder of consciousness in which patients with severe brain damage are in a state of partial arousal rather than true awareness. After four weeks in a vegetative state (VS), the patient is classified as being in a persistent vegetative state. This diagnosis is classified as a permanent vegetative state some months (three in the US and six in the UK) after a non-traumatic brain injury or one year after a traumatic injury. The term unresponsive wakefulness syndrome may be alternatively used,[2] as "vegetative state" has some negative connotations among the public.[3]
Persistent vegetative state | |
---|---|
Specialty | Neurology |
Definition
There are several definitions that vary by technical versus layman's usage. There are different legal implications in different countries.
Medical definition
Per the British Royal College of Physicians of London, a persistent vegetative state is "a wakeful unconscious state that lasts longer than a few weeks is referred to as a persistent (or 'continuing') vegetative state".[4]
"Vegetative state"
The vegetative state is a chronic or long-term condition. This condition differs from a coma: a coma is a state that lacks both awareness and wakefulness. Patients in a vegetative state may have awoken from a coma, but still have not regained awareness. In the vegetative state patients can open their eyelids occasionally and demonstrate sleep-wake cycles, but completely lack cognitive function. The vegetative state is also called a "coma vigil". The chances of regaining awareness diminish considerably as the time spent in the vegetative state increases.[5]
"Persistent vegetative state"
Persistent vegetative state is the standard usage (except in the UK) for a medical diagnosis, made after numerous neurological and other tests, that due to extensive and irreversible brain damage a patient is highly unlikely ever to achieve higher functions above a vegetative state. This diagnosis does not mean that a doctor has diagnosed improvement as impossible, but does open the possibility, in the US, for a judicial request to end life support.[6] Informal guidelines hold that this diagnosis can be made after four weeks in a vegetative state. US caselaw has shown that successful petitions for termination have been made after a diagnosis of a persistent vegetative state, although in some cases, such as that of Terri Schiavo, such rulings have generated widespread controversy.
In the UK, the term is discouraged in favor of two more precisely defined terms that have been strongly recommended by the Royal College of Physicians (RCP). These guidelines recommend using a continuous vegetative state for patients in a vegetative state for more than four weeks. A medical determination of a permanent vegetative state can be made if, after exhaustive testing and a customary 12 months of observation,[7] a medical diagnosis is made that it is impossible by any informed medical expectations that the mental condition will ever improve.[8] Hence, a "continuous vegetative state" in the UK may remain the diagnosis in cases that would be called "persistent" in the US or elsewhere.
While the actual testing criteria for a diagnosis of "permanent" in the UK are quite similar to the criteria for a diagnosis of "persistent" in the US, the semantic difference imparts in the UK a legal presumption that is commonly used in court applications for ending life support.[7] The UK diagnosis is generally only made after 12 months of observing a static vegetative state. A diagnosis of a persistent vegetative state in the US usually still requires a petitioner to prove in court that recovery is impossible by informed medical opinion, while in the UK the "permanent" diagnosis already gives the petitioner this presumption and may make the legal process less time-consuming.[6]
In common usage, the "permanent" and "persistent" definitions are sometimes conflated and used interchangeably. However, the acronym "PVS" is intended to define a "persistent vegetative state", without necessarily the connotations of permanence, and is used as such throughout this article. Bryan Jennett, who originally coined the term "persistent vegetative state", has now recommended using the UK division between continuous and permanent in his book The Vegetative State, arguing that "the 'persistent' component of this term ... may seem to suggest irreversibility".[9]
The Australian National Health and Medical Research Council has suggested "post coma unresponsiveness" as an alternative term for "vegetative state" in general.[10]
Lack of legal clarity
Unlike brain death, permanent vegetative state (PVS) is recognized by statute law as death in only a very few legal systems. In the US, courts have required petitions before termination of life support that demonstrate that any recovery of cognitive functions above a vegetative state is assessed as impossible by authoritative medical opinion.[11] In England, Wales and Scotland, the legal precedent for withdrawal of clinically assisted nutrition and hydration in cases of patients in a PVS was set in 1993 in the case of Tony Bland, who sustained catastrophic anoxic brain injury in the 1989 Hillsborough disaster.[4] An application to the Court of Protection is no longer required before nutrition and hydration can be withdrawn or withheld from PVS (or 'minimally conscious' – MCS) patients.[12]
This legal grey area has led to vocal advocates that those in PVS should be allowed to die. Others are equally determined that, if recovery is at all possible, care should continue. The existence of a small number of diagnosed PVS cases that have eventually resulted in improvement makes defining recovery as "impossible" particularly difficult in a legal sense.[6] This legal and ethical issue raises questions about autonomy, quality of life, appropriate use of resources, the wishes of family members, and professional responsibilities.
Signs and symptoms
Most PVS patients are unresponsive to external stimuli and their conditions are associated with different levels of consciousness. Some level of consciousness means a person can still respond, in varying degrees, to stimulation. A person in a coma, however, cannot. In addition, PVS patients often open their eyes in response to feeding, which has to be done by others; they are capable of swallowing, whereas patients in a coma subsist with their eyes closed.[13]
Cerebral cortical function (e.g. communication, thinking, purposeful movement, etc.) is lost while brainstem functions (e.g. breathing, maintaining circulation and hemodynamic stability, etc.) are preserved. Non-cognitive upper brainstem functions such as eye-opening, occasional vocalizations (e.g. crying, laughing), maintaining normal sleep patterns, and spontaneous non-purposeful movements often remain intact.
PVS patients' eyes might be in a relatively fixed position, or track moving objects, or move in a disconjugate (i.e., completely unsynchronized) manner. They may experience sleep-wake cycles, or be in a state of chronic wakefulness. They may exhibit some behaviors that can be construed as arising from partial consciousness, such as grinding their teeth, swallowing, smiling, shedding tears, grunting, moaning, or screaming without any apparent external stimulus.
Individuals in PVS are seldom on any life-sustaining equipment other than a feeding tube because the brainstem, the center of vegetative functions (such as heart rate and rhythm, respiration, and gastrointestinal activity) is relatively intact.[13]
Recovery
Many people emerge spontaneously from a vegetative state within a few weeks.[9] The chances of recovery depend on the extent of injury to the brain and the patient's age – younger patients having a better chance of recovery than older patients. A 1994 report found that of those who were in a vegetative state a month after a trauma, 54% had regained consciousness by a year after the trauma, whereas 28% had died and 18% were still in the vegetative state. For non-traumatic injuries such as strokes, only 14% had recovered consciousness at one year, 47% had died, and 39% were still vegetative. Patients who were vegetative six months after the initial event were much less likely to have recovered consciousness a year after the event than in the case of those who were simply reported vegetative at one month.[14] A New Scientist article from 2000 gives a pair of graphs[15] showing changes of patient status during the first 12 months after head injury and after incidents depriving the brain of oxygen.[16] After a year, the chances that a PVS patient will regain consciousness are very low[17] and most patients who do recover consciousness experience significant disability. The longer a patient is in a PVS, the more severe the resulting disabilities are likely to be. Rehabilitation can contribute to recovery, but many patients never progress to the point of being able to take care of themselves.
The medical literature also includes case reports of the recovery of a small number of patients following the removal of assisted respiration with cold oxygen.[18] The researchers found that in many nursing homes and hospitals unheated oxygen is given to non-responsive patients via tracheal intubation. This bypasses the warming of the upper respiratory tract and causes a chilling of aortic blood and chilling of the brain which the authors believe may contribute to the person's nonresponsive state. The researchers describe a small number of cases in which removal of the chilled oxygen was followed by recovery from the PVS and recommend either warming of oxygen with a heated nebulizer or removal of the assisted oxygen if it is no longer needed.[18] The authors further recommend additional research to determine if this chilling effect may either delay recovery or even may contribute to brain damage.
There are two dimensions of recovery from a persistent vegetative state: recovery of consciousness and recovery of function. Recovery of consciousness can be verified by reliable evidence of awareness of self and the environment, consistent voluntary behavioral responses to visual and auditory stimuli, and interaction with others. Recovery of function is characterized by communication, the ability to learn and to perform adaptive tasks, mobility, self-care, and participation in recreational or vocational activities. Recovery of consciousness may occur without functional recovery, but functional recovery cannot occur without recovery of consciousness.[19]
Causes
There are three main causes of PVS (persistent vegetative state):
- Acute traumatic brain injury
- Non-traumatic: neurodegenerative disorder or metabolic disorder of the brain
- Severe congenital abnormality of the central nervous system
Potential causes of PVS are:[20]
- Bacterial, viral, or fungal infection, including meningitis
- Increased intracranial pressure, such as a tumor or abscess
- Vascular pressure which causes intracranial hemorrhaging or stroke
- Hypoxic ischemic injury (hypotension, cardiac arrest, arrhythmia, near-drowning)
- Toxins such as uremia, ethanol, atropine, opiates, lead, dimethylmercury, endrin, parathion, and colloidal silver[21]
- Physical trauma: Concussion, contusion, etc.
- Seizure, both nonconvulsive status epilepticus and postconvulsive state (postictal state)
- Electrolyte imbalance, which involves hyponatremia, hypernatremia, hypomagnesemia, hypoglycemia, hyperglycemia, hypercalcemia, and hypocalcemia
- Postinfectious: Acute disseminated encephalomyelitis (ADEM)
- Endocrine disorders such as adrenal insufficiency and thyroid disorders
- Degenerative and metabolic diseases including urea cycle disorders, Reye syndrome, and mitochondrial disease
- Systemic infection and sepsis
- Hepatic encephalopathy
In addition, these authors claim that doctors sometimes use the mnemonic device AEIOU-TIPS to recall portions of the differential diagnosis: Alcohol ingestion and acidosis, Epilepsy and encephalopathy, Infection, Opiates, Uremia, Trauma, Insulin overdose or inflammatory disorders, Poisoning and psychogenic causes, and Shock.
Diagnosis
Despite converging agreement about the definition of persistent vegetative state, recent reports have raised concerns about the accuracy of diagnosis in some patients, and the extent to which, in a selection of cases, residual cognitive functions may remain undetected and patients are diagnosed as being in a persistent vegetative state. Objective assessment of residual cognitive function can be extremely difficult as motor responses may be minimal, inconsistent, and difficult to document in many patients, or may be undetectable in others because no cognitive output is possible.[22] In recent years, a number of studies have demonstrated an important role for functional neuroimaging in the identification of residual cognitive function in persistent vegetative state; this technology is providing new insights into cerebral activity in patients with severe brain damage. Such studies, when successful, may be particularly useful where there is concern about the accuracy of the diagnosis and the possibility that residual cognitive function has remained undetected.
Diagnostic experiments
Researchers have begun to use functional neuroimaging studies to study implicit cognitive processing in patients with a clinical diagnosis of persistent vegetative state. Activations in response to sensory stimuli with positron emission tomography (PET), functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), and electrophysiological methods can provide information on the presence, degree, and location of any residual brain function. However, use of these techniques in people with severe brain damage is methodologically, clinically, and theoretically complex and needs careful quantitative analysis and interpretation.
For example, PET studies have shown the identification of residual cognitive function in persistent vegetative state. That is, an external stimulation, such as a painful stimulus, still activates "primary" sensory cortices in these patients but these areas are functionally disconnected from "higher order" associative areas needed for awareness. These results show that parts of the cortex are indeed still functioning in "vegetative" patients.[23]
In addition, other PET studies have revealed preserved and consistent responses in predicted regions of auditory cortex in response to intelligible speech stimuli. Moreover, a preliminary fMRI examination revealed partially intact responses to semantically ambiguous stimuli, which are known to tap higher aspects of speech comprehension.[24]
Furthermore, several studies have used PET to assess the central processing of noxious somatosensory stimuli in patients in PVS. Noxious somatosensory stimulation activated midbrain, contralateral thalamus, and primary somatosensory cortex in each and every PVS patient, even in the absence of detectable cortical evoked potentials. In conclusion, somatosensory stimulation of PVS patients, at intensities that elicited pain in controls, resulted in increased neuronal activity in primary somatosensory cortex, even if resting brain metabolism was severely impaired. However, this activation of primary cortex seems to be isolated and dissociated from higher-order associative cortices.[25]
Also, there is evidence of partially functional cerebral regions in catastrophically injured brains. To study five patients in PVS with different behavioral features, researchers employed PET, MRI and magnetoencephalographic (MEG) responses to sensory stimulation. In three of the five patients, co-registered PET/MRI correlate areas of relatively preserved brain metabolism with isolated fragments of behavior. Two patients had had anoxic injuries and demonstrated marked decreases in overall cerebral metabolism to 30–40% of normal. Two other patients with non-anoxic, multifocal brain injuries demonstrated several isolated brain regions with higher metabolic rates, that ranged up to 50–80% of normal. Nevertheless, their global metabolic rates remained <50% of normal. MEG recordings from three PVS patients provide clear evidence for the absence, abnormality or reduction of evoked responses. Despite major abnormalities, however, these data also provide evidence for localized residual activity at the cortical level. Each patient partially preserved restricted sensory representations, as evidenced by slow evoked magnetic fields and gamma band activity. In two patients, these activations correlate with isolated behavioral patterns and metabolic activity. Remaining active regions identified in the three PVS patients with behavioral fragments appear to consist of segregated corticothalamic networks that retain connectivity and partial functional integrity. A single patient who sustained severe injury to the tegmental mesencephalon and paramedian thalamus showed widely preserved cortical metabolism, and a global average metabolic rate of 65% of normal. The relatively high preservation of cortical metabolism in this patient defines the first functional correlate of clinical–pathological reports associating permanent unconsciousness with structural damage to these regions. The specific patterns of preserved metabolic activity identified in these patients reflect novel evidence of the modular nature of individual functional networks that underlie conscious brain function. The variations in cerebral metabolism in chronic PVS patients indicate that some cerebral regions can retain partial function in catastrophically injured brains.[26]
Misdiagnoses
Statistical PVS misdiagnosis is common. An example study with 40 patients in the United Kingdom reported 43% of their patients classified as PVS were believed so and another 33% had recovered whilst the study was underway.[27] Some PVS cases may actually be a misdiagnosis of patients being in an undiagnosed minimally conscious state.[28] Since the exact diagnostic criteria of the minimally conscious state were only formulated in 2002, there may be chronic patients diagnosed as PVS before the secondary notion of the minimally conscious state became known.
Whether or not there is any conscious awareness with a patient's vegetative state is a prominent issue. Three completely different aspects of this should be distinguished. First, some patients can be conscious simply because they are misdiagnosed (see above). In fact, they are not in vegetative states. Second, sometimes a patient was correctly diagnosed but is then examined during the early stages of recovery. Third, perhaps some day the notion itself of vegetative states will change so to include elements of conscious awareness. Inability to disentangle these three example cases causes confusion. An example of such confusion is the response to an experiment using functional magnetic resonance imaging which revealed that a woman diagnosed with PVS was able to activate predictable portions of her brain in response to the tester's requests that she imagine herself playing tennis or moving from room to room in her house. The brain activity in response to these instructions was indistinguishable from those of healthy patients.[29][30][31]
In 2010, Martin Monti and fellow researchers, working at the MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit at the University of Cambridge, reported in an article in the New England Journal of Medicine[32] that some patients in persistent vegetative states responded to verbal instructions by displaying different patterns of brain activity on fMRI scans. Five out of a total of 54 diagnosed patients were apparently able to respond when instructed to think about one of two different physical activities. One of these five was also able to "answer" yes or no questions, again by imagining one of these two activities.[33] It is unclear, however, whether the fact that portions of the patients' brains light up on fMRI could help these patients assume their own medical decision making.[33]
In November 2011, a publication in The Lancet presented bedside EEG apparatus and indicated that its signal could be used to detect awareness in three of 16 patients diagnosed in the vegetative state.[34]
Treatment
Currently no treatment for vegetative state exists that would satisfy the efficacy criteria of evidence-based medicine. Several methods have been proposed which can roughly be subdivided into four categories: pharmacological methods, surgery, physical therapy, and various stimulation techniques. Pharmacological therapy mainly uses activating substances such as tricyclic antidepressants or methylphenidate. Mixed results have been reported using dopaminergic drugs such as amantadine and bromocriptine and stimulants such as dextroamphetamine.[35] Surgical methods such as deep brain stimulation are used less frequently due to the invasiveness of the procedures. Stimulation techniques include sensory stimulation, sensory regulation, music and musicokinetic therapy, social-tactile interaction, and cortical stimulation.[36]
Epidemiology
In the United States, it is estimated that there may be between 15,000 and 40,000 patients who are in a persistent vegetative state, but due to poor nursing home records exact figures are hard to determine.[39]
History
The syndrome was first described in 1940 by Ernst Kretschmer who called it apallic syndrome.[40] The term persistent vegetative state was coined in 1972 by Scottish spinal surgeon Bryan Jennett and American neurologist Fred Plum to describe a syndrome that seemed to have been made possible by medicine's increased capacities to keep patients' bodies alive.[9][41]
Society and culture
Ethics and policy
An ongoing debate exists as to how much care, if any, patients in a persistent vegetative state should receive in health systems plagued by limited resources. In a case before the New Jersey Superior Court, Betancourt v. Trinitas Hospital, a community hospital sought a ruling that dialysis and CPR for such a patient constitutes futile care. An American bioethicist, Jacob M. Appel, argued that any money spent treating PVS patients would be better spent on other patients with a higher likelihood of recovery.[42] The patient died naturally prior to a decision in the case, resulting in the court finding the issue moot.
In 2010, British and Belgian researchers reported in an article in the New England Journal of Medicine that some patients in persistent vegetative states actually had enough consciousness to "answer" yes or no questions on fMRI scans.[33] However, it is unclear whether the fact that portions of the patients' brains light up on fMRI will help these patient assume their own medical decision making.[33] Professor Geraint Rees, Director of the Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience at University College London, responded to the study by observing that, "As a clinician, it would be important to satisfy oneself that the individual that you are communicating with is competent to make those decisions. At the moment it is premature to conclude that the individual able to answer 5 out of 6 yes/no questions is fully conscious like you or I."[33] In contrast, Jacob M. Appel of the Mount Sinai Hospital told the Telegraph that this development could be a welcome step toward clarifying the wishes of such patients. Appel stated: "I see no reason why, if we are truly convinced such patients are communicating, society should not honour their wishes. In fact, as a physician, I think a compelling case can be made that doctors have an ethical obligation to assist such patients by removing treatment. I suspect that, if such individuals are indeed trapped in their bodies, they may be living in great torment and will request to have their care terminated or even active euthanasia."[33]
Notable cases
- Tony Bland – first patient in English legal history to be allowed to die
- Paul Brophy – first American to die after court-authorization
- Sunny von Bülow – lived almost 28 years in a persistent vegetative state until her death
- Gustavo Cerati – Argentine singer-songwriter, composer and producer who died after four years in a coma
- Prichard Colón – Puerto Rican former professional boxer and gold medal winner who spent years in a vegetative state after a bout
- Nancy Cruzan – American woman involved in a landmark United States Supreme Court case
- Gary Dockery – American police officer who entered, emerged and later reentered a persistent vegetative state
- Eluana Englaro – Italian woman from Lecco whose life was ended after a legal case after spending 17 years in a vegetative state
- Elaine Esposito – American woman who was a previous record holder for having spent 37 years in a coma
- Lia Lee – Hmong person who spent 26 years in a vegetative state and was the subject of a 1997 book by Anne Fadiman
- Martin Pistorius South African man who is a rare example of a survivor as his state progressed to minimally conscious after 3 years, locked in syndrome after another 4 more years, and fully came out of a coma after another 5 years. He is now a web designer, developer, and author. In 2011, he wrote a book called Ghost Boy, in which he describes his many years of being comatose.
- Annie Shapiro Canadian woman who is also another rare example of a survivor as it is known she couldn't think for her first 2 years of her 29 years total of being comatose. In 1992 she awakened fully recovered and lived her last 10 years peacefully. She is the longest a person has been in a coma and woken up apart from the catatonic stupor patients in Awakenings.
- Haleigh Poutre
- Karen Ann Quinlan
- Terri Schiavo
- Rita Greene
- Aruna Shanbaug – Indian woman in persistent vegetative state for 42 years until her death. Owing to her case, the Supreme Court of India allowed passive euthanasia in the country.
- Ariel Sharon
- Chayito Valdez
- Vice Vukov
- Helga Wanglie
- Otto Warmbier
See also
- Anencephaly
- Brain death
- Botulism
- Catatonia
- Karolina Olsson
- Locked-in syndrome
- Process Oriented Coma Work, for an approach to working with residual consciousness in patients in comatose and persistent vegetative states
References
- "Coma and brain injury". Synapse.org.
- Kang, Xiao-gang; Li, Li; Wei, Dong; Xu, Xiao-xia; Zhao, Rui; Jing, Yun-yun; Su, Ying-ying; Xiong, Li-ze; Zhao, Gang; Jiang, Wen (2014). "Development of a simple score to predict outcome for unresponsive wakefulness syndrome". Critical Care. 18 (1): R37. doi:10.1186/cc13745. ISSN 1364-8535. PMC 4056750. PMID 24571596.
- Laureys S, Celesia GG, Cohadon F, Lavrijsen J, León-Carrión J, Sannita WG, et al. (November 2010). "Unresponsive wakefulness syndrome: a new name for the vegetative state or apallic syndrome". BMC Medicine. 8: 68. doi:10.1186/1741-7015-8-68. PMC 2987895. PMID 21040571.
- "Prolonged Disorders of Consciousness: National Clinical Guidelines". Royal College of Physicians. 2013.
- Multi-Society Task Force on PVS (May 1994). "Medical aspects of the persistent vegetative state (1)". The New England Journal of Medicine. 330 (21): 1499–508. doi:10.1056/NEJM199405263302107. PMID 7818633.
- Cranford R (August 2004). "Diagnosing the permanent vegetative state". The Virtual Mentor. 6 (8). doi:10.1001/virtualmentor.2004.6.8.cprl1-0408. PMID 23260786.
- Wade DT, Johnston C (September 1999). "The permanent vegetative state: practical guidance on diagnosis and management". BMJ. 319 (7213): 841–44. doi:10.1136/bmj.319.7213.841. PMC 1116668. PMID 10496834.
- Guidance on diagnosis and management: Report of a working party of the Royal College of Physicians. Royal College of Physicians: London. 1996.
- Jennett B. The Vegetative State: Medical facts, ethical and legal dilemmas (PDF). Scotland: University of Glasgow. Retrieved 2007-11-09.
- Post-coma unresponsiveness (Vegetative State): a clinical framework for diagnosis. National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC): Canberra. 2003. Archived from the original on 2006-08-20.
- Jennett B (September 1999). "Should cases of permanent vegetative state still go to court?. Britain should follow other countries and keep the courts for cases of dispute". BMJ. 319 (7213): 796–97. doi:10.1136/bmj.319.7213.796. PMC 1116645. PMID 10496803.
- Royal College of Physicians 2013 Prolonged Disorders of Consciousness: National Clinical Guidelines
- Emmett PA (1989). A Biblico-Ethical Response to the Question of Withdrawing Fluid and Nutrition from Individuals in the Persistent Vegetative State (Master's thesis). Vol. 4–5. Bethel Theological Seminary. pp. 248–49.
- Jennett B (October 2002). "The vegetative state". Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 73 (4): 355–57. doi:10.1136/jnnp.73.4.355. PMC 1738081. PMID 12235296. Archived from the original on 2014-01-16.
- "New Scientist". 2014-02-02. Archived from the original on 2017-07-11. Retrieved 2019-01-07.
- Boyce N (July 2000). "Is anyone in there?". New Scientist. 167 (2246): 36–39. PMID 11995720.
- Schapira A (2006). Neurology and Clinical Neuroscience. Mosby. p. 126. ISBN 978-0-323-03354-1.
- Ford GP, Reardon DC (August 2006). "Prolonged unintended brain cooling may inhibit recovery from brain injuries: case study and literature review". Medical Science Monitor. 12 (8): CS74–79. PMID 16865070.
- Multi-Society Task Force on PVS (June 1994). "Medical aspects of the persistent vegetative state (2)". The New England Journal of Medicine. 330 (22): 1572–79. doi:10.1056/NEJM199406023302206. PMID 8177248.
- Teitelbaum JE, Kahan S, DeAntonis KO (2007). In a Page: Pediatric signs & symptoms (2nd ed.). Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. ISBN 978-0-7817-7045-3.
- Mirsattari SM, Hammond RR, Sharpe MD, Leung FY, Young GB (April 2004). "Myoclonic status epilepticus following repeated oral ingestion of colloidal silver". Neurology. 62 (8): 1408–10. doi:10.1212/01.WNL.0000120671.73335.EC. PMID 15111684. S2CID 30767207.
- Owen AM, Coleman MR, Menon DK, Johnsrude IS, Rodd JM, Davis MH, et al. (2005-07-01). "Residual auditory function in persistent vegetative state: a combined PET and fMRI study". Neuropsychological Rehabilitation. 15 (3–4): 290–306. doi:10.1080/09602010443000579. PMID 16350973. S2CID 7495756.
- Matsuda W, Matsumura A, Komatsu Y, Yanaka K, Nose T (November 2003). "Awakenings from persistent vegetative state: report of three cases with parkinsonism and brain stem lesions on MRI". Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 74 (11): 1571–73. doi:10.1136/jnnp.74.11.1571. PMC 1738238. PMID 14617720.
- Boly M, Faymonville ME, Peigneux P, Lambermont B, Damas P, Del Fiore G, et al. (February 2004). "Auditory processing in severely brain injured patients: differences between the minimally conscious state and the persistent vegetative state". Archives of Neurology. 61 (2): 233–38. doi:10.1001/archneur.61.2.233. PMID 14967772.
- Laureys S, Faymonville ME, Peigneux P, Damas P, Lambermont B, Del Fiore G, Degueldre C, Aerts J, Luxen A, Franck G, Lamy M, Moonen G, Maquet P (October 2002). "Cortical processing of noxious somatosensory stimuli in the persistent vegetative state". NeuroImage. 17 (2): 732–41. doi:10.1006/nimg.2002.1236. PMID 12377148. S2CID 16891079.
- Schiff ND, Ribary U, Moreno DR, Beattie B, Kronberg E, Blasberg R, et al. (June 2002). "Residual cerebral activity and behavioural fragments can remain in the persistently vegetative brain". Brain. 125 (Pt 6): 1210–34. doi:10.1093/brain/awf131. PMID 12023311.
- Andrews K, Murphy L, Munday R, Littlewood C (July 1996). "Misdiagnosis of the vegetative state: retrospective study in a rehabilitation unit". BMJ. 313 (7048): 13–16. doi:10.1136/bmj.313.7048.13. PMC 2351462. PMID 8664760.
- Giacino JT, Ashwal S, Childs N, Cranford R, Jennett B, Katz DI, et al. (February 2002). "The minimally conscious state: definition and diagnostic criteria". Neurology. 58 (3): 349–53. doi:10.1212/wnl.58.3.349. PMID 11839831.
- Owen AM, Coleman MR, Boly M, Davis MH, Laureys S, Pickard JD (September 2006). "Detecting awareness in the vegetative state". Science. 313 (5792): 1402. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.1022.2193. doi:10.1126/science.1130197. PMID 16959998. S2CID 54524352.
- "Vegetative patient 'communicates': A patient in a vegetative state can communicate just through using her thoughts, according to research". BBC News. September 7, 2006. Retrieved 2008-08-14.
- Stein R (September 8, 2006). "Vegetative patient's brain active in test: Unprecedented experiment shows response to instructions to imagine playing tennis". San Francisco Chronicle. Retrieved 2007-09-26.
- Monti MM, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Coleman MR, Boly M, Pickard JD, Tshibanda L, Owen AM, Laureys S (18 February 2010). "Willful Modulation of Brain Activity in Disorders of Consciousness". New England Journal of Medicine. 362 (7): 579–89. doi:10.1056/NEJMoa0905370. PMID 20130250. S2CID 13358991.
- Alleyne R, Beckford M (4 February 2010). "Patients in 'vegetative' state can think and communicate". Telegraph. United Kingdom. Archived from the original on 6 February 2010.
- Cruse D, Chennu S, Chatelle C, Bekinschtein TA, Fernández-Espejo D, Pickard JD, et al. (December 2011). "Bedside detection of awareness in the vegetative state: a cohort study". Lancet. 378 (9809): 2088–94. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.368.3928. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(11)61224-5. PMID 22078855. S2CID 1926221.
- Dolce G, Sazbon L (2002). The post-traumatic vegetative state. ISBN 978-1-58890-116-3.
- Georgiopoulos M, Katsakiori P, Kefalopoulou Z, Ellul J, Chroni E, Constantoyannis C (2010). "Vegetative state and minimally conscious state: a review of the therapeutic interventions". Stereotactic and Functional Neurosurgery. 88 (4): 199–207. doi:10.1159/000314354. PMID 20460949.
- Snyman N, Egan JR, London K, Howman-Giles R, Gill D, Gillis J, Scheinberg A (October 2010). "Zolpidem for persistent vegetative state--a placebo-controlled trial in pediatrics". Neuropediatrics. 41 (5): 223–37. doi:10.1055/s-0030-1269893. PMID 21210338.
- Whyte J, Myers R (May 2009). "Incidence of clinically significant responses to zolpidem among patients with disorders of consciousness: a preliminary placebo controlled trial". American Journal of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation. 88 (5): 410–18. doi:10.1097/PHM.0b013e3181a0e3a0. PMID 19620954. S2CID 19666318.
- Hirsch J (May 2005). "Raising consciousness". The Journal of Clinical Investigation. 115 (5): 1102. doi:10.1172/JCI25320. PMC 1087197. PMID 15864333.
- Kretschmer E (1940). "Das apallische Syndrom". Zeitschrift für die gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie. 169: 576–79. doi:10.1007/BF02871384.
- Jennett B, Plum F (April 1972). "Persistent vegetative state after brain damage. A syndrome in search of a name". Lancet. 1 (7753): 734–37. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(72)90242-5. PMID 4111204.
- Pope TM (23 June 2010). "Appel on Betancourt v. Trinitas Hospital". Medical Futility Blog.
This article contains text from the NINDS public domain pages on TBI. Archived 2005-10-19 at the Wayback Machine and .
Further reading
- Sarà M, Sacco S, Cipolla F, Onorati P, Scoppetta C, Albertini G, Carolei A (January 2007). "An unexpected recovery from permanent vegetative state". Brain Injury. 21 (1): 101–03. doi:10.1080/02699050601151761. PMID 17364525. S2CID 22730610.
- Canavero S, Massa-Micon B, Cauda F, Montanaro E (May 2009). "Bifocal extradural cortical stimulation-induced recovery of consciousness in the permanent post-traumatic vegetative state". Journal of Neurology. 256 (5): 834–36. doi:10.1007/s00415-009-5019-4. PMID 19252808. S2CID 17087007.
- Canavero S, ed. (2009). Textbook of therapeutic cortical stimulation. New York: Nova Science. ISBN 978-1-60692-537-9.
- Canavero S, Massa-Micon B, Cauda F, Montanaro E (May 2009). "Bifocal extradural cortical stimulation-induced recovery of consciousness in the permanent post-traumatic vegetative state". Journal of Neurology. 256 (5): 834–36. doi:10.1007/s00415-009-5019-4. PMID 19252808. S2CID 17087007.
- Connolly K (23 November 2009). "Car crash victim trapped in a coma for 23 years was conscious". The Guardian.
- Machado C, Estévez M, Rodríguez R, Pérez-Nellar J, Gutiérrez J, Carballo M, Olivares A, Fleitas M, Pando A, Beltrán C (January 2012). "A Cuban perspective on management of persistent vegetative state". MEDICC Review. 14 (1): 44–48. doi:10.37757/MR2012V14.N1.3. PMID 22334112.