Spanish phonology

This article is about the phonology and phonetics of the Spanish language. Unless otherwise noted, statements refer to Castilian Spanish, the standard dialect used in Spain on radio and television.[1][2][3][4] For historical development of the sound system, see History of Spanish. For details of geographical variation, see Spanish dialects and varieties.

Phonemes are written inside slashes (/ /) and allophones inside brackets ([ ]).

Consonants

Consonant phonemes[5]
Labial Dental Alveolar Post-alv./
Palatal
Velar
Nasal m n ɲ
Stop pb td ʝ kɡ
Continuant f θ* s (ʃ) x
Lateral l ʎ*
Tap ɾ
Trill r

The phonemes /b/, /d/, and /ɡ/ are pronounced as voiced stops only after a pause, after a nasal consonant, or—in the case of /d/—after a lateral consonant; in all other contexts, they are realized as approximants (namely [β̞, ð̞, ɣ˕], hereafter represented without the downtacks) or fricatives.[6][7]

The realization of the phoneme /ʝ/ varies greatly by dialect.[8] In Castilian Spanish, its allophones in word-initial position include the palatal approximant [j], the palatal fricative [ʝ], the palatal affricate [ɟʝ] and the palatal stop [ɟ].[8] After a pause, a nasal, or a lateral, it may be realized as an affricate ([ɟʝ]);[9][10] in other contexts, /ʝ/ is generally realized as an approximant [ʝ˕].

The phoneme /ʎ/ is distinguished from /ʝ/ in some areas in Spain (mostly northern and rural) and South America (mostly highland). Other accents of Spanish, comprising the majority of speakers, have lost the palatal lateral as a distinct phoneme and have merged historical /ʎ/ into /ʝ/: this is called yeísmo.

Many young Argentinians have no distinct /ɲ/ phoneme and use the /ni/ sequence instead, thus making no distinction between huraño and uranio (both [uˈɾanjo]).[11]

Most varieties spoken in Spain, including those prevalent on radio and television, have both /θ/ and /s/ (distinción). However, speakers in parts of southern Spain, the Canary Islands, and nearly all of Latin America have only /s/ (seseo). Some speakers in southernmost Spain (especially coastal Andalusia) have only [] (a consonant similar to /θ/) and not /s/ (ceceo). This "ceceo" is not entirely unknown in the Americas, especially in coastal Peru. The word distinción itself is pronounced with /θ/ in varieties that have it.

The exact pronunciation of /s/ varies widely by dialect, with some realizing it as [h] or opting to omit it entirely [∅].[12]

The phonemes /t/ and /d/ are laminal denti-alveolar ([, ]).[7] The phoneme /s/ becomes dental [s̪] before denti-alveolar consonants,[9] while /θ/ remains interdental [θ̟] in all contexts.[9]

Before front vowels /i, e/, the velar consonants /k, ɡ, x/ (including the lenited allophone of /ɡ/) are realized as post-palatal [, ɡ˖, , ɣ˕˖].[13]

According to some authors,[14] /x/ is post-velar or uvular in the Spanish of northern and central Spain.[15][16][17][18] Others[19] describe /x/ as velar in European Spanish, with a uvular allophone ([χ]) appearing before /o/ and /u/ (including when /u/ is in the syllable onset as [w]).[9]

A common pronunciation of /f/ in nonstandard speech is the voiceless bilabial fricative [ɸ], so that fuera is pronounced [ˈɸweɾa] rather than [ˈfweɾa].[20][21][22][23][24][25][26] In some Extremaduran, western Andalusian, and American varieties, this softened realization of /f/, when it occurs before the non-syllabic allophone of /u/ ([w]), is subject to merger with /x/; in some areas the homophony of fuego/juego is resolved by replacing fuego with lumbre or candela.[27][28]

/ʃ/ is a marginal phoneme that occurs only in loanwords or certain dialects; many speakers have difficulty with this sound, tending to replace it with /tʃ/ or /s/. In a number of dialects (most notably, Northern Mexican Spanish, informal Chilean Spanish, and some Caribbean and Andalusian accents) [ʃ] occurs, as a deaffricated /tʃ/.[21] In addition, [ʃ] occurs in Rioplatense Spanish as spoken across Argentina and Uruguay, where it is otherwise standard for the phonemes /ʝ/ to be realized as voiced palato-alveolar fricative [ʒ] instead of [ʝ], a feature called "zheísmo".[29] In the last few decades, it has further become popular, particularly among younger speakers in Argentina and Uruguay, to de-voice /ʒ/ to [ʃ] ("sheísmo").[30][31]

Consonant neutralizations and assimilations

Some of the phonemic contrasts between consonants in Spanish are lost in certain phonological environments, especially in syllable-final position. In these cases, the phonemic contrast is said to be neutralized.

Nasals and laterals

The three nasal phonemes/m/, /n/, and /ɲ/—maintain their contrast when in syllable-initial position (e.g. cama 'bed', cana 'grey hair', caña 'sugar cane'). In syllable-final position, this three-way contrast is lost as nasals assimilate to the place of articulation of the following consonant[9]—even across a word boundary;[32] or, if a nasal is followed by a pause rather than a consonant, it is realized for most speakers as alveolar [n] (though in Caribbean varieties, this may instead be [ŋ] or an omitted nasal with nasalization of the preceding vowel).[33][34] Thus /n/ is realized as [m] before labial consonants, and as [ŋ] before velar ones. Additionally, word-final /m/ and /ɲ/ in stand-alone loanwords or proper nouns are substituted with [n], e.g. álbum [ˈalβun] ('album').[35]

Similarly, /l/ assimilates to the place of articulation of a following coronal consonant, i.e. a consonant that is interdental, dental, alveolar, or palatal.[36][37][38] In dialects that maintain the use of /ʎ/, there is no contrast between /ʎ/ and /l/ in coda position, and syllable-final [ʎ] appears only as an allophone of /l/ in rapid speech.[39]

Assimilatory nasal and lateral allophones in Spanish
nasal lateral
word IPA gloss word IPA gloss
invierno [imˈbjeɾno] 'winter'
ánfora[ˈaɱfoɾa]'amphora'
encía[en̟ˈθi.a]'gum'alzar[al̟ˈθaɾ]'to raise'
antes[ˈan̪t̪es]'before'alto[ˈal̪t̪o]'tall'
ancha[ˈanʲtʃa]'wide'colcha[ˈkolʲtʃa]'quilt'
nyuge[ˈkoɲɟʝuxe]'spouse'
rincón[riŋˈkon]'corner'
enjuto[eɴˈχut̪o]'thin'
Rhotics

The alveolar trill [r] and the alveolar tap [ɾ] are in phonemic contrast word-internally between vowels (as in carro 'car' vs caro 'expensive'), but are otherwise in complementary distribution, as long as syllable division is taken into account: the tap occurs after any syllable-initial consonant, while the trill occurs after any syllable-final consonant.[40][41]

Only the trill can occur at the start of a morpheme (e.g. el rey 'the king', la reina 'the queen') or at the start of a syllable when the preceding syllable ends with a consonant, namely /l/, /n/, or /s/ (e.g. alrededor, enriquecer, Israel).

Only the tap can occur after a word-initial obstruent consonant (e.g. tres 'three', frío 'cold').

Either a trill or a tap can be found word-medially after /b/, /d/, /t/ depending on whether the rhotic consonant is pronounced in the same syllable as the preceding obstruent (forming a complex onset cluster) or in a separate syllable (with the obstruent forming the coda of the preceding syllable). The tap is found in words where no morpheme boundary separates the obstruent from the following rhotic consonant, such as sobre 'over', madre 'mother', ministro 'minister'. The trill is found in words where the rhotic consonant is preceded by a morpheme boundary and thus a syllable boundary, such as subrayar, ciudadrealeño, postromántico;[42] compare the corresponding word-initial trills in raya 'line', Ciudad Real "Ciudad Real", and romántico "Romantic".

In syllable-final position inside a word, the tap is more frequent, but the trill can also occur (especially in emphatic[43] or oratorical[44] style) with no semantic difference—thus arma ('weapon') may be either [ˈaɾma] (tap) or [ˈarma] (trill).[45] In word-final position the rhotic is usually:

  • either a tap or a trill when followed by a consonant or a pause, as in amo ~ r] paterno ('paternal love'), the former being more common;[46]
  • a tap when followed by a vowel-initial word, as in amo[ɾ] eterno ('eternal love').

Morphologically, a word-final rhotic always corresponds to the tapped [ɾ] in related words. Thus the word olor 'smell' is related to olores, oloroso 'smells, smelly' and not to *olorres, *olorroso.[8]

When two rhotics occur consecutively across a word or prefix boundary, they result in one trill, so that da rocas ('s/he gives rocks') and dar rocas ('to give rocks') are either neutralized or distinguished by a longer trill in the latter phrase.[47]

The tap/trill alternation has prompted a number of authors to postulate a single underlying rhotic; the intervocalic contrast then results from gemination (e.g. tierra /ˈtieɾɾa/ > [ˈtjera] 'earth').[48][49][50]

Obstruents

The phonemes /θ/, /s/,[9] and /f/[51][52] may be voiced before voiced consonants as in jazmín ('Jasmine') [xaðˈmin], rasgo ('feature') [ˈrazɣo], and Afganistán ('Afghanistan') [avɣanisˈtan]. There is a certain amount of free variation in this so that jazmín can be pronounced [xaθˈmin] or [xaðˈmin].[53] This may occur across word boundaries, causing feliz navidad ('merry Christmas') /feˈliθ nabiˈdad/ to be pronounced [feˈlið naβ̞iˈð̞að̞].[12] In one region of Spain, the area around Madrid, word-final /d/ is sometimes pronounced [θ] especially in a colloquial pronunciation of its name, Madriz ([maˈðɾiθ]).[54] (In some words now spelled with -z- before a voiced consonant, the phoneme /θ/ is in fact diachronically derived from original [ð] or /d/. For example, yezgo comes from Old Spanish yedgo and juzgar comes from Old Spanish judgar, from Latin jūdicāre.[55])

Both in casual and formal speech, there is no phonemic contrast between voiced and voiceless consonants placed in syllable-final position. The merged phoneme is typically pronounced as a relaxed, voiced fricative or approximant,[56] although a variety of other realizations are also possible. So the clusters -bt- and -pt- in the words obtener and optimista are pronounced exactly the same way:

  • obtener /obteˈner/ > [oβteˈneɾ]
  • optimista /obtiˈmista/ > [oβtiˈmista]

Similarly, the spellings -dm- and -tm- are often merged in pronunciation, as well as -gd- and -cd-:

Semivowels

Traditionally, the palatal consonant phoneme /ʝ/ is considered to occur only as a syllable onset,[57] whereas the palatal glide [j] that can be found after a consonantal onset in words like bien is analyzed as a non-syllabic version of the vowel phoneme /i/[58] (which forms part of the syllable nucleus, being pronounced with the following vowel as a rising diphthong). The approximant allophone of /ʝ/, which can be transcribed as [ʝ˕], differs phonetically from [j] in the following respects: [ʝ˕] has a lower F2 amplitude, is longer, can be replaced by a palatal fricative [ʝ] in emphatic pronunciations, and is unspecified for rounding (e.g. viuda [ˈbjuða] 'widow' vs ayuda [aˈʝʷuða] 'help').[59]

After a consonant, the surface contrast between [ʝ] and [j] depends on syllabification, which in turn is largely predictable from morphology: the syllable boundary before [ʝ] corresponds to the morphological boundary after a prefix.[8] A contrast is therefore possible after any consonant that can end a syllable, as illustrated by the following minimal or near-minimal pairs: after /l/ (italiano [itaˈljano] 'Italian' vs. y tal llano [italˈʝano] 'and such a plain'[8]), after /n/ (enyesar [eɲɟʝeˈsaɾ] 'to plaster' vs. aniego [aˈnjeɣo] 'flood'[10]) after /s/ (desierto /deˈsieɾto/ 'desert' vs. deshielo /desˈʝelo/ 'thawing'[8]), after /b/ (abierto /aˈbierto/ 'open' vs. abyecto /abˈʝekto/ 'abject'[8][60]).

Although there is dialectal and ideolectal variation, speakers may also exhibit a contrast in phrase-initial position.[61] In Argentine Spanish, the change of /ʝ/ to a fricative realized as /ʒ/ ~ /ʃ/ has resulted in clear contrast between this consonant and the glide /j/; the latter occurs as a result of spelling pronunciation in words spelled with hi, such as hierba /jerba/ 'grass' (which thus forms a minimal pair in Argentine Spanish with the doublet yerba /ʒerba/ 'maté leaves').[62]

There are some alternations between the two, prompting scholars like Alarcos Llorach (1950)[63] to postulate an archiphoneme /I/, so that ley [lej] would be transcribed phonemically as /ˈleI/ and leyes [ˈleʝes] as /ˈleIes/.

In a number of varieties, including some American ones, there is a similar distinction between the non-syllabic version of the vowel /u/ and a rare consonantal /w̝/.[10][64] Near-minimal pairs include deshuesar [dez.w̝eˈsaɾ] ('to debone') vs. desuello [deˈsweʝo] ('skinning'), son huevos [ˈsoŋ ˈw̝eβos] ('they are eggs') vs son nuevos [ˈso(n) ˈnweβos] ('they are new'),[65] and huaca [ˈ(ɡ)w̝aka] ('Indian grave') vs u oca [ˈwoka] ('or goose').[66]

Vowels

Spanish vowel chart, from Ladefoged & Johnson (2010:227)
Front Central Back
Close iu
Mid eo
Open a

Spanish has five vowel phonemes, /i/, /u/, /e/, /o/ and /a/ (the same as Asturian-Leonese, Aragonese, and also Basque).

There is no phonemic distinction between close-mid and open-mid vowels, unlike in Catalan, French, Italian and Portuguese. The open-mid vowel phonemes /ɛ, ɔ/ of the aforementioned languages correspond to the Spanish opening diphthongs /ie, ue/ (with /ʝe/ occurring instead of /ie/ in word-initial position): for example, Spanish siete /ˈsiete/ 'seven' and fuerte /ˈfuerte/ 'strong' are cognate with Portuguese sete /ˈsɛtɨ/ and forte /ˈfɔɾtɨ/, meaning the same.[67] The diphthongs /ie, ue/ in stressed syllables show alternation with the monophthongs /e, o/ in unstressed syllables: compare heló /eˈlo/ 'it froze' and tostó /tosˈto/ 'he toasted' with hiela /ˈʝela/ 'it freezes' and tuesto /ˈtuesto/ 'I toast'.[68]

Each of the five vowels in both stressed and unstressed syllables:[69]

Examples of Spanish vowels
stressed unstressed
word gloss word gloss
piso/ˈpiso/'I step'pi/piˈso/'s/he stepped'
pujo/ˈpuxo/'I bid' (present tense)pu/puˈxo/'s/he bid'
peso/ˈpeso/'I weigh'pe/peˈso/'s/he weighed'
poso/ˈposo/'I pose'po/poˈso/'s/he posed'
paso/ˈpaso/'I pass'pa/paˈso/'s/he passed'

Nevertheless, there are some distributional gaps or rarities. For instance, an unstressed close vowel in the final syllable of a word is rare.[70]

Because of substratal Quechua, at least some speakers from southern Colombia down through Peru can be analyzed to have only three vowel phonemes /i, u, a/, as the close [i, u] are continually confused with the mid [e, o], resulting in pronunciations such as [dolˈsoɾa] for dulzura ('sweetness'). When Quechua-dominant bilinguals have /e, o/ in their phonemic inventory, they realize them as [ɪ, ʊ], which are heard by outsiders as variants of /i, u/.[71] Both of those features are viewed as strongly non-standard by other speakers.

Allophones

Phonetic nasalization occurs for vowels occurring between nasal consonants or when preceding a syllable-final nasal, e.g. cinco [ˈθĩŋko] ('five') and mano [ˈmãno] ('hand').[69]

Arguably, Eastern Andalusian and Murcian Spanish have ten phonemic vowels, with each of the above vowels paired by a lowered or fronted and lengthened version, e.g. la madre [la ˈmaðɾe] ('the mother') vs. las madres [læː ˈmæːðɾɛː] ('the mothers').[72] However, these are more commonly analyzed as allophones triggered by an underlying /s/ that is subsequently deleted.

Exact number of allophones

There is no agreement among scholars on how many vowel allophones Spanish has; an often[73] postulated number is five [i, u, , , ].

Some scholars,[74] however, state that Spanish has eleven allophones: the close and mid vowels have close [i, u, e, o] and open [ɪ, ʊ, ɛ, ɔ] allophones, whereas /a/ appears in front [a], central [] and back [ɑ] variants. These symbols appear only in the narrowest variant of phonetic transcription; in broader variants, only the symbols i, u, e, o, a are used,[75] and that is the convention adopted in the rest of this article.

Tomás Navarro Tomás describes the distribution of said eleven allophones as follows:[76]

  • Close vowels /i, u/
    • The close allophones [i, u] appear in open syllables, e.g. in the words libre [ˈliβɾe] 'free' and subir [suˈβɪɾ] 'to raise'
    • The open allophones are phonetically near-close [ɪ, ʊ], and appear:
      • In closed syllables, e.g. in the word fin [fɪn] 'end'
      • In both open and closed syllables when in contact with /r/, e.g. in the words rico [ˈrɪko] 'rich' and rubio [ˈrʊβjo] 'blond'
      • In both open and closed syllables when before /x/, e.g. in the words hijo [ˈɪxo] 'son' and pu [pʊˈxo] 's/he bid'
  • Mid front vowel /e/
    • The close allophone is phonetically close-mid [e], and appears:
      • In open syllables, e.g. in the word dedo [ˈdeðo] 'finger'
      • In closed syllables when before /m, n, t, θ, s/, e.g. in the word Valencia [ba̠ˈlenθja̠] 'Valencia'
    • The open allophone is phonetically open-mid [ɛ], and appears:
      • In open syllables when in contact with /r/, e.g. in the words guerra [ˈɡɛra̠] 'war' and reto [ˈrɛto] challenge
      • In closed syllables when not followed by /m, n, t, θ, s/, e.g. in the word belga [ˈbɛlɣa̠] 'Belgian'
      • In the diphthong /ei/, e.g. in the words peine [ˈpɛjne] 'comb' and rey [ˈrɛj] king
  • Mid back vowel /o/
    • The close allophone is phonetically close-mid [o], and appears in open syllables, e.g. in the word como [ˈkomo] 'how'
    • The open allophone is phonetically open-mid [ɔ], and appears:
      • In closed syllables, e.g. in the word con [kɔn] 'with'
      • In both open and closed syllables when in contact with /r/, e.g. in the words corro [ˈkɔrɔ] 'I run', barro [ˈba̠rɔ] 'mud', and roble [ˈrɔβle] 'oak'
      • In both open and closed syllables when before /x/, e.g. in the word ojo [ˈɔxo] 'eye'
      • In the diphthong /oi/, e.g. in the word hoy [ɔj] 'today'
      • In stressed position when preceded by /a/ and followed by either /ɾ/ or /l/, e.g. in the word ahora [ɑˈɔɾa̠] 'now'
  • Open vowel /a/
    • The front allophone [a] appears:
      • Before palatal consonants, e.g. in the word despacho [desˈpatʃo] 'office'
      • In the diphthong /ai/, e.g. in the word aire [ˈajɾe] 'air'
    • The back allophone [ɑ] appears:
      • Before the back vowels /u, o/, e.g. in the word flauta [ˈflɑwta̠] 'flute'
      • In closed syllables before /l/, e.g. in the word sal [sɑl] 'salt'
      • In both open and closed syllables when before /x/, e.g. in the word tajada [tɑˈxa̠ða̠] 'chop'
    • The central allophone [] appears in all other cases, e.g. in the word casa [ˈka̠sa̠]

According to Eugenio Martínez Celdrán, however, systematic classification of Spanish allophones is impossible due to the fact that their occurrence varies from speaker to speaker and from region to region. According to him, the exact degree of openness of Spanish vowels depends not so much on the phonetic environment, but rather on various external factors accompanying speech.[77]

Diphthongs and triphthongs

Spanish diphthongs[69]
IPA Example Meaning IPA Example Meaning
Falling Rising
a [aj]aireair [ja]haciatowards
[aw]pausapause [wa]cuadropicture
e [ej]reyking [je]tierraearth
[ew]neutroneutral [we]fuegofire
o [oj]hoytoday [jo]radioradio
[ow][78]bouseine fishing [wo]cuotaquota
Falling Rising
u/i [ju]viudawidow
[uj][79]muyvery [wi]buitrevulture

Spanish has six falling diphthongs and eight rising diphthongs. While many diphthongs are historically the result of a recategorization of vowel sequences (hiatus) as diphthongs, there is still lexical contrast between diphthongs and hiatus.[80] There are also some lexical items that vary amongst speakers and dialects between hiatus and diphthong: words like biólogo ('biologist') with a potential diphthong in the first syllable and words like diálogo with a stressed or pretonic sequence of /i/ and a vowel vary between a diphthong and hiatus.[81] Chițoran & Hualde (2007) hypothesize that this is because vocalic sequences are longer in these positions.

In addition to synalepha across word boundaries, sequences of vowels in hiatus become diphthongs in fast speech; when this happens, one vowel becomes non-syllabic (unless they are the same vowel, in which case they fuse together) as in poeta [ˈpo̯eta] ('poet') and maestro [ˈmae̯stɾo] ('teacher').[82] Similarly, the relatively rare diphthong /eu/ may be reduced to [u] in certain unstressed contexts, as in Eufemia, [uˈfemja].[83] In the case of verbs like aliviar ('relieve'), diphthongs result from the suffixation of normal verbal morphology onto a stem-final /j/ (that is, aliviar would be |alibj| + |ar|).[84] This contrasts with verbs like ampliar ('to extend') which, by their verbal morphology, seem to have stems ending in /i/.[85]

Non-syllabic /e/ and /o/ can be reduced to [j], [w], as in beatitud [bjatiˈtuð] ('beatitude') and poetisa [pweˈtisa] ('poetess'), respectively; similarly, non-syllabic /a/ can be completely elided, as in (e.g. ahorita [oˈɾita] 'right away'). The frequency (though not the presence) of this phenomenon differs amongst dialects, with a number having it occur rarely and others exhibiting it always.[86]

Spanish also possesses triphthongs like /uei/ and, in dialects that use a second person plural conjugation, /iai/, /iei/, and /uai/ (e.g. buey, 'ox'; cambiáis, 'you change'; cambiéis, '(that) you may change'; and averiguáis, 'you ascertain').[87]

Prosody

Spanish is usually considered a syllable-timed language. Even so, stressed syllables can be up to 50% longer in duration than non-stressed syllables.[88][89][90] Although pitch, duration, and loudness contribute to the perception of stress,[91] pitch is the most important in isolation.[92]

Primary stress occurs on the penultima (the next-to-last syllable) 80% of the time. The other 20% of the time, stress falls on the ultima (last syllable) or on the antepenultima (third-to-last syllable).[93]

Nonverbs are generally stressed on the penultimate syllable for vowel-final words and on the final syllable of consonant-final words. Exceptions are marked orthographically (see below), whereas regular words are underlyingly phonologically marked with a stress feature [+stress].[94]

In addition to exceptions to these tendencies, particularly learned words from Greek and Latin that feature antepenultimate stress, there are numerous minimal pairs which contrast solely on stress such as sábana ('sheet') and sabana ('savannah'), as well as límite ('boundary'), limite ('[that] he/she limit') and limité ('I limited').

Lexical stress may be marked orthographically with an acute accent (ácido, distinción, etc.). This is done according to the mandatory stress rules of Spanish orthography, which are similar to the tendencies above (differing with words like distinción) and are defined so as to unequivocally indicate where the stress lies in a given written word. An acute accent may also be used to differentiate homophones, such as mi (my), and (me). In such cases, the accent is used on the homophone that normally receives greater stress when used in a sentence.

Lexical stress patterns are different between words carrying verbal and nominal inflection: in addition to the occurrence of verbal affixes with stress (something absent in nominal inflection), underlying stress also differs in that it falls on the last syllable of the inflectional stem in verbal words while those of nominal words may have ultimate or penultimate stress.[95] In addition, amongst sequences of clitics suffixed to a verb, the rightmost clitic may receive secondary stress, e.g. búscalo /ˈbuskaˌlo/ ('look for it').[96]

Alternations

A number of alternations exist in Spanish that reflect diachronic changes in the language and arguably reflect morphophonological processes rather than strictly phonological ones. For instance, a number of words alternate between /k/ and /θ/ or /ɡ/ and /x/, with the latter in each pair appearing before a front vowel:[97]

Examples of Spanish alternations
word gloss word gloss
opaco/oˈpako/'opaque'opacidad/opaθiˈdad/'opacity'
sueco/ˈsweko/'Swedish'Suecia/ˈsweθja/'Sweden'
belga/ˈbelɡa/'Belgian'Bélgica/ˈbelxika/'Belgium'
análogo/aˈnaloɡo/'analogous'analogía/analoˈxi.a/'analogy'

Note that the conjugation of most verbs with a stem ending in /k/ or /ɡ/ does not show this alternation; these segments do not turn into /θ/ or /x/ before a front vowel:

word gloss word gloss
seco/ˈseko/'I dry'seque/ˈseke/'(that) I/he/she dry (subjunctive)'
castigo/kasˈtiɡo/'I punish'castigue/kasˈtiɡe/'(that) I/he/she punish (subjunctive)'

There are also alternations between unstressed /e/ and /o/ and stressed /ie/ (or /ʝe/, when initial) and /ue/ respectively:[98]

word gloss word gloss
he/eˈlo/'it froze'hiela/ˈʝela/'it freezes'
tostó/tosˈto/'he toasted'tuesto/ˈtuesto/'I toast'

Likewise, in a very small number of words, alternations occur between the palatal sonorants ɲ/ and their corresponding alveolar sonorants /l n/ (doncella/doncel 'maiden'/'youth', desdeñar/desdén 'to scorn'/'scorn'). This alternation does not appear in verbal or nominal inflection (that is, the plural of doncel is donceles, not *doncelles).[99] This is the result of geminated /ll/ and /nn/ of Vulgar Latin (the origin of /ʎ/ and /ɲ/, respectively) degeminating and then depalatalizing in coda position.[100] Words without any palatal-alveolar allomorphy are the result of historical borrowings.[100]

Other alternations include /ɡs/ ~ /x/ (anexo vs anejo),[101] /ɡt/ ~ /tʃ/ (nocturno vs noche).[102] Here the forms with /ɡs/ and /ɡt/ are historical borrowings and the forms with /x/ and /tʃ/ forms are inherited from Vulgar Latin.

There are also pairs that show antepenultimate stress in nouns and adjectives but penultimate stress in synonymous verbs (vómito 'vomit' vs. vomito 'I vomit').[103]

Phonotactics

Spanish syllable structure can be summarized as follows; parentheses enclose optional components:

  • (C1 (C2)) (S1) V (S2) (C3 (C4))

Spanish syllable structure consists of an optional syllable onset, consisting of one or two consonants; an obligatory syllable nucleus, consisting of a vowel optionally preceded by and/or followed by a semivowel; and an optional syllable coda, consisting of one or two consonants.[104] The following restrictions apply:

  • Onset
    • First consonant (C1): Can be any consonant, including a liquid (/l, ʎ, ɾ, r/).
      • However, as discussed above, the contrast between the two rhotic consonants is neutralized in some contexts: either /ɾ/ and /r/ is possible as a word-internal onset when the preceding syllable ends in a vowel, but they do not contrast in word-initial position or when the preceding syllable ends in a consonant: only /r/ is a possible onset in those positions. In native Spanish words, the trill /r/ does not appear after a glide.[8] That said, it does appear after [w] in some Basque loans, such as Aurrerá, a grocery store, Abaurrea Alta and Abaurrea Baja, towns in Navarre, aurresku, a type of dance, and aurragado, an adjective referring to poorly tilled land.[8]
      • In general, the palatal sonorants /ʎ, ɲ/ are not found as word-internal onsets when the preceding syllable ends in a coda consonant or glide.[105] However, the diphthong [au̯] is found before /ɲ/ in some proper names, such as the toponym Auñón (from Latin alneus[106]) and Auñamendi (a publishing house name taken from from the Basque name of the Pic d'Anie).
    • Second consonant (C2): If and only if the first consonant is a stop /p, t, k, b, d, ɡ/ or a voiceless labiodental fricative /f/, a second consonant, either /l/ or /ɾ/, is permitted. The onset /dl/ is nonexistent. /tl/ is prohibited as an onset cluster in most of Peninsular Spanish, while /tl/ sequences such as in atleta 'athlete' are usually treated as an onset cluster in Latin America and the Canaries.[104][107][108]
  • Nucleus
    • Semivowel (S1)
    • Vowel (V)
    • Semivowel (S2)
  • Coda
    • First consonant (C3): Can be any consonant except /ɲ/, /ʝ/ or /ʎ/.[104]
    • Second consonant (C4): Always /s/ in native Spanish words.[104] Other consonants, except /ɲ/, /ʝ/ and /ʎ/, are tolerated as long as they are less sonorous than the first consonant in the coda, such as in York or the Catalan last name Brucart, though sometimes the final element is deleted in colloquial speech.[109] A coda of two consonants never appears in words inherited from Vulgar Latin.
    • Medial codas assimilate place features of the following onsets and are often stressed.[110]

Maximal onsets include transporte /tɾansˈpor.te/, flaco /ˈfla.ko/, clave /ˈkla.be/.

Maximal nuclei include buey /buei/, Uruguay /u.ɾuˈɡuai/.

Maximal codas include instalar /ins.taˈlar/, perspectiva /pers.peɡˈti.ba/.

In many dialects, a coda cannot be more than one consonant (one of n, r, l or s) in informal speech. Realizations like /tɾasˈpor.te/, /is.taˈlar/, /pes.peɡˈti.ba/ are very common, and in many cases, they are allowed even in formal speech.

Epenthesis

Because of the phonotactic constraints, an epenthetic /e/ is inserted before word-initial clusters beginning with /s/ (e.g. escribir 'to write') but not word-internally (transcribir 'to transcribe'),[111] thereby moving the initial /s/ to a separate syllable. The epenthetic /e/ is pronounced even when it is not reflected in spelling (e.g. the surname of Carlos Slim is pronounced /esˈlin/). While Spanish words undergo word-initial epenthesis, cognates in Latin and Italian do not:

  • Lat. status /ˈsta.tus/ ('state') ~ It. stato /ˈsta.to/ ~ Sp. estado /esˈta.do/
  • Lat. splendidus /ˈsplen.di.dus/ ('splendid') ~ It. splendido /ˈsplen.di.do/ ~ Sp. espléndido /esˈplen.di.do/
  • Fr. slave /slav/ ('Slav') ~ It. slavo /ˈzla.vo/ ~ Sp. eslavo /esˈla.bo/

In addition, Spanish adopts foreign words starting with pre-nasalized consonants with an epenthetic /e/. Nguema, a prominent last name from Equatorial Guinea, is pronounced as [eŋˈɡema].[112]

When adapting word-final complex codas that show rising sonority, an epenthetic /e/ is inserted between the two consonants. For example, al Sadr is typically pronounced [al.sa.ðeɾ].[113]

Occasionally Spanish speakers are faced with onset clusters containing elements of equal or near-equal sonority, such as Knoll (a German last name, common in parts of South America). Assimilated borrowings usually delete the first element in such clusters, for example (p)sicología 'psychology'. When attempting to pronounce such words for the first time without deleting the first consonant, Spanish speakers insert a short, often devoiced, schwa-like svarabhakti vowel between the two consonants.[114]

Spanish syllable structure is phrasal, resulting in syllables consisting of phonemes from neighboring words in combination, sometimes even resulting in elision. The phenomenon is known in Spanish as enlace.[115] For a brief discussion contrasting Spanish and English syllable structure, see Whitley (2002:32–35).

Acquisition as a first language

Phonology

Phonological development varies greatly by individual, both those developing regularly and those with delays. However, a general pattern of acquisition of phonemes can be inferred by the level of complexity of their features, i.e. by sound classes.[116] A hierarchy may be constructed, and if a child is capable of producing discrimination on one level, they will also be capable of making the discriminations of all prior levels.[117]

  • The first level consists of stops (without a voicing distinction), nasals, [l], and optionally, a non-lateral approximant. This includes a labial/coronal place difference (for example, [b] vs [t] and [l] vs [β]).
  • The second level includes voicing distinction for oral stops and a coronal/dorsal place difference. This allows for a distinction between [p], [t], and [k], along with their voiced counterparts, as well as a distinction between [l] and the approximant [j].
  • The third level includes fricatives and/or affricates.
  • The fourth level introduces liquids other than [l], [ɹ] and [ɾ]. It also introduces [θ].
  • The fifth level introduces the trill [r].

This hierarchy is based on production only, and is a representation of a child's capacity to produce a sound, whether that sound is the correct target in adult speech or not. Thus, it may contain some sounds that are not included in adult phonology but are produced as a result of error.

Spanish-speaking children will accurately produce most segments at a relatively early age. By around three-and-a-half years, they will no longer productively use phonological processes the majority of the time. Some common error patterns (found 10% or more of the time) are cluster reduction, liquid simplification, and stopping. Less common patterns (evidenced less than 10% of the time) include palatal fronting, assimilation, and final consonant deletion.[118]

Typical phonological analyses of Spanish consider the consonants /b/, /d/, and /ɡ/ the underlying phonemes and their corresponding approximants [β], [ð], and [ɣ] allophonic and derivable by phonological rules. However, approximants may be the more basic form because monolingual Spanish-learning children learn to produce the continuant contrast between [p t k] and ð ɣ] before they do the lead voicing contrast between [p t k] and [b d ɡ].[119] (In comparison, English-learning children are able to produce adult-like voicing contrasts for these stops well before age three.)[120] The allophonic distribution of [b d ɡ] and ð ɣ] produced in adult speech is not learned until after age two and not fully mastered even at age four.[119]

The alveolar trill [r] is one of the most difficult sounds to produce in Spanish and, as a result, is acquired later in development.[121] Research suggests that the alveolar trill is acquired and developed between the ages of three and six years.[122] Some children acquire an adult-like trill within this period, and some fail to properly acquire the trill. The attempted trill sound of the poor trillers is often perceived as a series of taps owing to hyperactive tongue movement during production.[123]

The trill is also often very difficult for those learning Spanish as a second language, sometimes taking over a year to be produced properly.[124]

Codas

One research study found that children acquire medial codas before final codas, and stressed codas before unstressed codas.[125] Since medial codas are often stressed and must undergo place assimilation, greater importance is accorded to their acquisition.[110] Liquid and nasal codas occur word-medially and at the ends of frequently used function words, so they are often acquired first.[126]

Prosody

Research suggests that children overgeneralize stress rules when they are reproducing novel Spanish words and that they have a tendency to stress the penultimate syllables of antepenultimately stressed words, to avoid a violation of nonverb stress rules that they have acquired.[127] Many of the most frequent words heard by children have irregular stress patterns or are verbs, which violate nonverb stress rules.[128] This complicates stress rules until ages three to four, when stress acquisition is essentially complete, and children begin to apply these rules to novel irregular situations.

Dialectal variation

Some features, such as the pronunciation of voiceless stops /p t k/, have no dialectal variation.[129] However, there are numerous other features of pronunciation that differ from dialect to dialect.

Yeísmo

One notable dialectal feature is the merging of the voiced palatal approximant [ʝ] (as in ayer) with the palatal lateral approximant [ʎ] (as in calle) into one phoneme (yeísmo), with /ʎ/ losing its laterality. While the distinction between these two sounds has traditionally been a feature of Castilian Spanish, this merger has spread throughout most of Spain in recent generations, particularly outside of regions in close linguistic contact with Catalan and Basque.[130] In Spanish America, most dialects are characterized by this merger, with the distinction persisting mostly in parts of Peru, Bolivia, Paraguay, and northwestern Argentina.[131] In the other parts of Argentina, the phoneme resulting from the merger is realized as [ʒ];[9] and in Buenos Aires the sound has recently been devoiced to [ʃ] among the younger population; the change is spreading throughout Argentina.[132]

Seseo, ceceo and distinción

Most dialects in Spanish from Spain have [s] / [θ] contrast (distinción), while [θ] is absent in Latin America and parts of Spain (seseo).

Speakers in northern and central Spain, including the variety prevalent on radio and television, have both /θ/ and /s/ (distinción, 'distinction'). However, speakers in Latin America, Canary Islands and some parts of southern Spain have only /s/ (seseo), which in southernmost Spain is pronounced [θ] and not [s] (ceceo).[9]

Realization of /s/

The phoneme /s/ has three different pronunciations depending on the dialect area:[9][37][133]

  1. An apical alveolar retracted fricative (or "apico-alveolar" fricative) [s̺], which sounds similar to English /ʃ/ and is characteristic of the northern and central parts of Spain and is also used by many speakers in Colombia's Antioquia department.[134][135]
  2. A laminal alveolar grooved fricative [s], much like the most common pronunciation of English /s/, is characteristic of western Andalusia (e.g. Málaga, Seville, and Cádiz), the Canary Islands, and Latin America.
  3. An apical dental grooved fricative [s̄] (ad hoc symbol), which has a lisping quality and sounds something like a cross between English /s/ and /θ/ but is different from the /θ/ occurring in dialects that distinguish /s/ and /θ/. It occurs only in dialects with ceceo, mostly in Granada, in parts of Jaén, in the southern part of Sevilla and the mountainous areas shared between Cádiz and Málaga.

Obaid describes the apico-alveolar sound as follows:[136]

There is a Castilian s, which is a voiceless, concave, apicoalveolar fricative: the tip of the tongue turned upward forms a narrow opening against the alveoli of the upper incisors. It resembles a faint /ʃ/ and is found throughout much of the northern half of Spain.

Dalbor describes the apico-dental sound as follows:[137]

[s̄] is a voiceless, corono-dentoalveolar groove fricative, the so-called s coronal or s plana because of the relatively flat shape of the tongue body ... To this writer, the coronal [s̄], heard throughout Andalusia, should be characterized by such terms as "soft," "fuzzy," or "imprecise," which, as we shall see, brings it quite close to one variety of /θ/ ... Canfield has referred, quite correctly, in our opinion, to this [s̄] as "the lisping coronal-dental," and Amado Alonso remarks how close it is to the post-dental [θ̦], suggesting a combined symbol θˢ̣ to represent it.

In some dialects, /s/ may become the approximant [ɹ] in the syllable coda (e.g. doscientos [doɹˈθjentos] 'two hundred').[138] In southern dialects in Spain, most lowland dialects in the Americas, and in the Canary Islands, it debuccalizes to [h] in final position (e.g. niños [ˈniɲoh] 'children'), or before another consonant (e.g. fósforo [ˈfohfoɾo] 'match') so the change occurs in the coda position in a syllable. In Spain, this was originally a southern feature, but it is now expanding rapidly to the north.[28]

From an autosegmental point of view, the /s/ phoneme in Madrid is defined only by its voiceless and fricative features. Thus, the point of articulation is not defined and is determined from the sounds following it in the word or sentence. In Madrid, the following realizations are found: /pesˈkado/ > [pexˈkao][139] and /ˈfosfoɾo/ > [ˈfofːoɾo]. In parts of southern Spain, the only feature defined for /s/ appears to be voiceless; it may lose its oral articulation entirely to become [h] or even a geminate with the following consonant ([ˈmihmo] or [ˈmimːo] from /ˈmismo/ 'same').[140] In Eastern Andalusian and Murcian Spanish, word-final /s/, /θ/ and /x/ regularly weaken, and the preceding vowel is lowered and lengthened:[141]

/is/ > [ɪː] e.g. mis [mɪː] ('my' pl)
/es/ > [ɛː] e.g. mes [mɛː] ('month')
/as/ > [æː] e.g. más [mæː] ('plus')
/os/ > [ɔː] e.g. tos [tɔː] ('cough')
/us/ > [ʊː] e.g. tus [tʊː] ('your' pl)

A subsequent process of vowel harmony takes place so lejos ('far') is [ˈlɛxɔ], tenéis ('you [plural] have') is [tɛˈnɛj] and tréboles ('clovers') is [ˈtɾɛβɔlɛ] or [ˈtɾɛβolɛ].[142]

Coda simplification

Southern European Spanish (Andalusian Spanish, Murcian Spanish, etc.) and several lowland dialects in Latin America (such as those from the Caribbean, Panama, and the Atlantic coast of Colombia) exhibit more extreme forms of simplification of coda consonants:

  • word-final dropping of /s/ (e.g. compás [komˈpa] 'musical beat' or 'compass')
  • word-final dropping of nasals with nasalization of the preceding vowel (e.g. ven [bẽ] 'come')
  • dropping of /r/ in the infinitival morpheme (e.g. comer [koˈme] 'to eat')
  • the occasional dropping of coda consonants word-internally (e.g. doctor [doˈto(r)] 'doctor').[143]

The dropped consonants appear when additional suffixation occurs (e.g. compases [komˈpase] 'beats', venían [beˈni.ã] 'they were coming', comeremos [komeˈɾemo] 'we will eat'). Similarly, a number of coda assimilations occur:

  • /l/ and /r/ may neutralize to [j] (e.g. Cibaeño Dominican celda/cerda [ˈsejða] 'cell'/'bristle'), to [l] (e.g. Caribbean Spanish alma/arma [ˈalma] 'soul'/'weapon', Andalusian Spanish sartén [salˈtẽ] 'pan'), to [r] (e.g. Andalusian Spanish alma/arma [ˈarma]) or, by complete regressive assimilation, to a copy of the following consonant (e.g. pulga/purga [ˈpuɡːa] 'flea'/'purge', carne [ˈkanːe] 'meat').[143]
  • /s/, /x/, (and /θ/ in southern Peninsular Spanish) and /f/ may be debuccalized or elided in the coda (e.g. los amigos [lo(h) aˈmiɣo(h)] 'the friends').[144]
  • Stops and nasals may be realized as velar (e.g. Cuban and Venezuelan étnico [ˈeɡniko] 'ethnic', himno [ˈiŋno] 'anthem').[144]

Final /d/ dropping (e.g. mitad [miˈta] 'half') is general in most dialects of Spanish, even in formal speech.[145]

The neutralization of syllable-final /p/, /t/, and /k/ is widespread in most dialects (with e.g. Pepsi being pronounced [ˈpeksi]). It does not face as much stigma as other neutralizations, and may go unnoticed.[146]

The deletions and neutralizations show variability in their occurrence, even with the same speaker in the same utterance, so nondeleted forms exist in the underlying structure.[147] The dialects may not be on the path to eliminating coda consonants since deletion processes have been existing for more than four centuries.[148] Guitart (1997) argues that it is the result of speakers acquiring multiple phonological systems with uneven control like that of second language learners.

In Standard European Spanish, the voiced obstruents /b, d, ɡ/ before a pause are devoiced and laxed to [β̥˕, ð̥˕, ɣ̊˕], as in club [kluβ̥˕] ('[social] club'), sed [seð̥] ('thirst'), zigzag [θiɣˈθaɣ̊˕].[149] However, word-final /b/ is rare, and /ɡ/ even more so. They are restricted mostly to loanwords and foreign names, such as the first name of former Real Madrid sports director Predrag Mijatović, which is pronounced [ˈpɾeð̞ɾaɣ̊˕]; and after another consonant, the voiced obstruent may even be deleted, as in iceberg, pronounced [iθeˈβeɾ].[150] In Madrid and its environs, sed is alternatively pronounced [seθ], where the aforementioned alternative pronunciation of word-final /d/ as [θ] coexists with the standard realization,[151] but is otherwise nonstandard.[54]

Loan sounds

The fricative /ʃ/ may also appear in borrowings from other languages, such as Nahuatl[152] and English.[153] In addition, the affricates /t͡s/ and /t͡ɬ/ also occur in Nahuatl borrowings.[152] That said, the onset cluster /tl/ is permitted in most of Latin America, the Canaries, and the northwest of Spain, and the fact that it is pronounced in the same amount of time as the other voiceless stop + lateral clusters /pl/ and /kl/ support an analysis of the /tl/ sequence as a cluster rather than an affricate in Mexican Spanish.[107][108]

Sample

This sample is an adaptation of Aesop's "El Viento del Norte y el Sol" (The North Wind and the Sun) read by a man from Northern Mexico born in the late 1980s. As usual in Mexican Spanish, /θ/ and /ʎ/ are not present.

Orthographic version

El Viento del Norte y el Sol discutían por saber quién era el más fuerte de los dos. Mientras discutían, se acercó un viajero cubierto en un cálido abrigo. Entonces decidieron que el más fuerte sería quien lograse despojar al viajero de su abrigo. El Viento del Norte empezó, soplando tan fuerte como podía, pero entre más fuerte soplaba, el viajero más se arropaba. Entonces, el Viento desistió. Se llegó el turno del Sol, quien comenzó a brillar con fuerza. Esto hizo que el viajero sintiera calor y por ello se quitó su abrigo. Entonces el Viento del Norte tuvo que reconocer que el Sol era el más fuerte de los dos.

Phonemic transcription

/el ˈbiento del ˈnoɾte i el ˈsol diskuˈti.an poɾ saˈbeɾ ˈkien ˈeɾa el ˈmas ˈfueɾte de los ˈdos mientɾas diskuˈti.an se aseɾˈko un biaˈxeɾo kuˈbieɾto en un ˈkalido aˈbɾiɡo | enˈtonses desiˈdieɾon ke el ˈmas ˈfueɾte seˈɾi.a kien loˈɡɾase despoˈxaɾ al biaˈxeɾo de su aˈbɾiɡo el ˈbiento del ˈnoɾte empeˈso soˈplando tan ˈfueɾte komo poˈdi.a | peɾo entɾe ˈmas ˈfueɾte soˈplaba el biaˈxeɾo ˈmas se aroˈpaba | enˈtonses el ˈbiento desisˈtio | se ʝeˈɡo el ˈtuɾno del ˈsol kien komenˈso a bɾiˈʝaɾ kon ˈfueɾsa | ˈesto ˈiso ke el biaˈxeɾo sinˈtieɾa kaˈloɾ i poɾ ˈeʝo se kiˈto su aˈbɾiɡo enˈtonses el ˈbiento del ˈnoɾte ˈtubo ke rekonoˈseɾ ke el ˈsol ˈeɾa el ˈmas ˈfueɾte de los ˈdos/

Phonetic transcription

[el ˈβjento ðel ˈnoɾte j‿el ˈsol diskuˈti.am por saˈβeɾ ˈkjen eɾa‿e̯l ˈmas ˈfweɾte ðe los ˈðos ˈmjentɾas ðiskuˈti.an ˌse̯‿aseɾˈko‿wm bjaˈxeɾo kuˈβjeɾto̯‿en ˈkaliðo̯‿aˈβɾiɣo | enˈtonses ðesiˈðjeɾoŋ k‿el ˈmas ˈfweɾte seˈɾi.a kjen loˈɣɾase ðespoˈxaɾ al βjaˈxeɾo ðe swaˈβɾiɣo el ˈβjento ðel ˌnoɾt‿empeˈso soˈplando taɱ ˈfweɾte ˌkomo poˈði.a | ˈpeɾo̯‿entɾe ˈmas ˈfweɾte soˈplaβa el βjaˈxeɾo ˈmas ˌse̯‿aroˈpaβa | enˈtonses el ˈβjento ðesisˈtjo | se ʝeˈɣo̯‿el ˈtuɾno ðel sol ˌkjeŋ komenˈso̯‿a βɾiˈʝar koɱ ˈfweɾsa | ˈesto‿jso k‿el βjaxeɾo sinˈtjeɾa kaˈloɾ i poɾ eʝo se kiˈto swaˈβɾiɣo enˈtonses el ˈβjento ðel ˈnoɾte ˈtuβo ke rekonoˈseɾ ˌkel ˈsol ˈeɾa‿e̯l ˈmas ˈfweɾte ðe los ˈðos]

See also

Notes

  1. Random House Unabridged Dictionary, Random House Inc., 2006
  2. The American Heritage Dictionary of the English Language (4th ed.), Houghton Mifflin Company, 2006
  3. Webster's Revised Unabridged Dictionary, MICRA, Inc., 1998
  4. Encarta World English Dictionary. Bloomsbury Publishing Plc. 2007. Archived from the original on 2009-11-09. Retrieved 2008-08-05.
  5. Martínez Celdrán, Fernández Planas & Carrera Sabaté (2003:255)
  6. The continuant allophones of Spanish /b, d, ɡ/ have been traditionally described as voiced fricatives (e.g. Navarro Tomás (1918), who (in §100) describes the air friction of [ð] as being "tenue y suave" ('weak and smooth'); Harris (1969); Dalbor (1997); and Macpherson (1975:62), who describes [β] as being "...with audible friction"). However, they are more often described as approximants in recent literature, such as D'Introno, Del Teso & Weston (1995); Martínez Celdrán, Fernández Planas & Carrera Sabaté (2003); and Hualde (2005:43). The difference hinges primarily on air turbulence caused by extreme narrowing of the opening between articulators, which is present in fricatives and absent in approximants. Martínez Celdrán (2004) displays a sound spectrogram of the Spanish word abogado showing an absence of turbulence for all three consonants.
  7. Martínez Celdrán, Fernández Planas & Carrera Sabaté (2003:257)
  8. Hualde, José Ignacio (2005). "Quasi-phonemic contrasts in Spanish". WCCFL 23: Proceedings of the 23rd West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press. pp. 374–398.
  9. Martínez Celdrán, Fernández Planas & Carrera Sabaté (2003:258)
  10. Trager (1942:222)
  11. Coloma (2018:245)
  12. Núñez-Méndez, Eva (June 2022). "Variation in Spanish /s/: Overview and New Perspectives". Languages. 7 (2): 77. doi:10.3390/languages7020077. ISSN 2226-471X.
  13. Canellada & Madsen (1987:20–21)
  14. For example Chen (2007), Hammond (2001) and Lyons (1981)
  15. Chen (2007:13)
  16. Hammond (2001:?), cited in Scipione & Sayahi (2005:128)
  17. Harris & Vincent (1988:83)
  18. Lyons (1981:76)
  19. such as Martínez Celdrán, Fernández Planas & Carrera Sabaté (2003)
  20. Boyd-Bowman (1953:229)
  21. Cotton & Sharp (1988:15)
  22. Flórez (1951:171)
  23. Kany (1960:236)
  24. Lenz (1940:92 et seq.)
  25. Zamora Vicente (1967:413)
  26. Zapata Arellano (1975)
  27. Mott (2011:110)
  28. Penny (2000:122)
  29. Chang (2008), p. 54.
  30. Chang (2008), p. 55.
  31. Staggs, Cecelia (2019). "A Perception Study of Rioplatense Spanish". McNair Scholars Research Journal. Boise State University. 14 (1). Many studies have shown that within the last 70 to 80 years, there has been a strong transition towards the voiceless [ʃ] in both Argentina and Uruguay, with Argentina having completed the change by 2004 and Uruguay following only recently [...]
  32. Cressey (1978:61)
  33. MacDonald (1989:219)
  34. Lipski (1994:?)
  35. "5.3. Nasal (nasales)". Teaching Spanish Pronunciation. OpenLearn Create. The distribution of nasals, however, is somewhat deficient in Spanish. In word-final position only the alveolar nasal is present. So borrowings that end in /ɲ/ or /m/ are generally adopted into Spanish with a final n, e.g. Adam -> Adán, champagne -> champán.
  36. Navarro Tomás (1918:§111, 113)
  37. Dalbor (1980)
  38. D'Introno, Del Teso & Weston (1995:118–121)
  39. Navarro Tomás (1918:§125)
  40. Hooper (1972:527)
  41. Lipski (1990:155)
  42. Sorbet (2018:73)
  43. D'Introno, Del Teso & Weston (1995:294)
  44. Canfield (1981:13)
  45. Harris (1969:56)
  46. Hualde (2005:182–3)
  47. Hualde (2005:184).
  48. Bowen, Stockwell & Silva-Fuenzalida (1956)
  49. Harris (1969)
  50. Bonet & Mascaró (1997)
  51. Harris (1969:37 n.)
  52. D'Introno, Del Teso & Weston (1995:289)
  53. Cotton & Sharp (1988:19)
  54. Salgado, Cristóbal González (2012). Eñe B1.2: der Spanischkurs. Hueber Verlag. p. 91. ISBN 978-3-19-004294-4. Archived from the original on 5 August 2020.
  55. Dworkin, Steven N. (1978). "Derivational Transparency and Sound Change: The Two-Pronged Growth of -ǏDU in Hispano-Romance". Romance Philology. 31 (4): 613. Retrieved 4 October 2023.
  56. Navarro Tomás (1918, §98, §125)
  57. Martínez Celdrán (2004:208)
  58. Bowen & Stockwell (1955:236)
  59. Martínez Celdrán (2004:208)
  60. Saporta (1956:288)
  61. Bowen & Stockwell (1955:236) cite the minimal pair ya visto [(ɟ)ʝa ˈβisto] ('I already dress') vs y ha visto [ja ˈβisto] ('and he has seen')
  62. Scarpace, Beery & Hualde (2015:92)
  63. cited in Saporta (1956:289)
  64. Generally /w̝/ is [ɣʷ] though it may also be [βˠ] (Ohala & Lorentz (1977:590) citing Navarro Tomás (1961) and Harris (1969)).
  65. Saporta (1956:289)
  66. Bowen & Stockwell (1955:236)
  67. Ulsh (1971), pp. 10, 12.
  68. Harris (1969), pp. 26–27.
  69. Martínez Celdrán, Fernández Planas & Carrera Sabaté (2003:256)
  70. Harris (1969:78, 145). Examples include words of Greek origin like énfasis /ˈenfasis/ ('emphasis'); the clitics su /su/, tu /tu/, mi /mi/; the three Latin words espíritu /esˈpiɾitu/ ('spirit'), tribu /ˈtɾibu/ ('tribe'), and ímpetu /ˈimpetu/ ('impetus'); and affective words like mami /ˈmami/ and papi /ˈpapi/.
  71. Cotton & Sharp (1988:182)
  72. Zamora Vicente (1967:?). The first /a/ in madres also undergoes this fronting process as part of a vowel harmony system. See #Realization of /s/ below.
  73. See e.g. Martínez Celdrán, Fernández Planas & Carrera Sabaté (2003)
  74. Such as Navarro Tomás (1918)
  75. Nowikow (2012:16)
  76. Navarro Tomás (1918), cited on Joaquim Llisterri's site
  77. Martínez Celdrán (1984:289, 294, 301)
  78. /ou/ occurs rarely in words; another example is the proper name Bousoño (Saporta 1956, p. 290). It is, however, common across word boundaries as with tengo una casa ('I have a house').
  79. Harris (1969:89) points to muy ('very') as the one example with [uj] rather than [wi]. There are also a handful of proper nouns with [uj], exclusive to Chuy (a nickname) and Ruy. There are no minimal pairs.
  80. Chițoran & Hualde (2007:45)
  81. Chițoran & Hualde (2007:46)
  82. Martínez Celdrán, Fernández Planas & Carrera Sabaté (2003:256–257)
  83. Cotton & Sharp (1988:18)
  84. Harris (1969:99–101).
  85. See Harris (1969:147–148) for a more extensive list of verb stems ending in both high vowels, as well as their corresponding semivowels.
  86. Bowen & Stockwell (1955:237)
  87. Saporta (1956:290)
  88. Navarro Tomás (1916)
  89. Navarro Tomás (1917)
  90. Quilis (1971)
  91. Cotton & Sharp (1988:19–20)
  92. García-Bellido (1997:492), citing Contreras (1963), Quilis (1971), and the Esbozo de una nueva gramática de la lengua española. (1973) by the Gramática de la Real Academia Española.
  93. Lleó (2003:262)
  94. Hochberg (1988:684)
  95. García-Bellido (1997:473–474)
  96. García-Bellido (1997:486), citing Navarro Tomás (1917:381–382, 385)
  97. Harris (1969:79)
  98. Harris (1969:26–27)
  99. Pensado (1997:595–597)
  100. Pensado (1997:608)
  101. Harris (1969:188)
  102. Harris (1969:189)
  103. Harris (1969:97)
  104. Lipski (2016:245)
  105. Baker, Gary Kenneth (2004). Palatal phenomena in Spanish phonology (PDF) (Thesis). University of Florida. p. 30.
  106. Menéndez Pidal, Ramón (1926). Orígenes del español. Estado lingüístico de la Península Ibérica hasta el siglo XI. Madrid: Librería y Casa Editorial Hernando. p. 121.
  107. Hualde, José Ignacio; Carrasco, Patricio (2009). "/tl/ en español mexicano. ¿Un segmento o dos?" (PDF). Estudios de Fonética Experimental (in Spanish). XVIII: 175–191. ISSN 1575-5533.
  108. "División silábica y ortográfica de palabras con "tl"". Real Académia Española (in Spanish). Retrieved 19 July 2021.
  109. Lipski (2016:249–250)
  110. Lleó (2003:278)
  111. Cressey (1978:86)
  112. Lipski (2016:252)
  113. Lipski (2016:250)
  114. Lipski (2016:254)
  115. "Enlace / Encadenamiento - Lawless Spanish Pronunciation". 25 September 2020.
  116. Cataño, Barlow & Moyna (2009:456)
  117. Cataño, Barlow & Moyna (2009:448)
  118. Goldstein & Iglesias (1998:5–6)
  119. Macken & Barton (1980b:455)
  120. Macken & Barton (1980b:73)
  121. Carballo & Mendoza (2000:588)
  122. Carballo & Mendoza (2000:589)
  123. Carballo & Mendoza (2000:596)
  124. Leibowitz, Brandon (11 February 2015). "Spanish Phonology". Fluency Fox. Archived from the original on 23 April 2016. Retrieved 5 April 2016.
  125. Lleó (2003:271)
  126. Lleó (2003:279)
  127. Hochberg (1988:683)
  128. Hochberg (1988:685)
  129. Cotton & Sharp (1988:55)
  130. Coloma (2011:110–111)
  131. Coloma (2011:95)
  132. Lipski (1994:170)
  133. Obaid (1973)
  134. Flórez (1957:41)
  135. Canfield (1981:36)
  136. Obaid (1973).
  137. Dalbor (1980:9).
  138. Recasens (2004:436) citing Fougeron (1999) and Browman & Goldstein (1995)
  139. Wright, Robyn (2017). The Madrileño ejke : a study of the perception and production of velarized /s/ in Madrid (PhD). The University of Texas at Austin. hdl:2152/60470. OCLC 993940787.
  140. Obaid (1973:62)
  141. Zamora Vicente (1967:?)
  142. Lloret (2007:24–25)
  143. Guitart (1997:515)
  144. Guitart (1997:517)
  145. Lipski (1997:124)
  146. Lipski (1997:126)
  147. Guitart (1997:515, 517–518)
  148. Guitart (1997:518, 527), citing Boyd-Bowman (1975) and Labov (1994:595)
  149. Wetzels & Mascaró (2001:224) citing Navarro Tomás (1961)
  150. The Oxford Spanish Dictionary (Oxford University Press, 1994).
  151. Molina Martos, Isabel (2016). "Variación de la -/d/ final de palabra en Madrid: ¿prestigio abierto o encubierto?". Boletín de Filología. 51 (2): 347–367. doi:10.4067/S0718-93032016000200013. ISSN 0718-9303.
  152. Lope Blanch (2004:29)
  153. Ávila (2003:67)

References

Further reading

This article is issued from Wikipedia. The text is licensed under Creative Commons - Attribution - Sharealike. Additional terms may apply for the media files.